• Title/Summary/Keyword: affective perspective taking

Search Result 9, Processing Time 0.02 seconds

Cognitive and Affective Perspective-Taking Ability of Young Bilinguals in South Korea

  • Han, Sinae;Lee, Kangyi
    • Child Studies in Asia-Pacific Contexts
    • /
    • v.3 no.1
    • /
    • pp.69-80
    • /
    • 2013
  • The present study examined balanced bilingual children's cognitive and affective perspective-taking and compared them to that of monolingual children. A total of 133 children aged 4 to 5 years and consisting of 73 Korean-English bilinguals and 60 Korean monolinguals were tested with cognitive perspective-taking and affective perspective-taking tasks. Balanced bilinguals were screened through general language ability tests in both English and Korean. Participant backgrounds were collected through a parent questionnaire. Results showed significant differences in affective perspective-taking between bilingual and monolingual children, demonstrating that bilingual children outperformed monolingual children. Although there was no difference in cognitive perspective-taking between bilinguals and monolinguals, the result showed that children's cognitive perspective-taking ability develops with age. This study provides basic information about bilingual children's perspective-taking ability and their bilingual advantage.

A Comparative Study on the Orphanage and Normal Children's Affective, Cognitive Perspective-taking Ability (시설아동과 일반아동의 정서적, 인지적 조망수용능력에 관한 비교 연구)

  • Park, Kyoung-Oak;Lee, Kyung-Nim
    • Korean Journal of Human Ecology
    • /
    • v.6 no.1
    • /
    • pp.1-14
    • /
    • 1997
  • The purpose of this study was to comprehend the age-related differences in the relationship and developmental tendency between Orphanage and Normal Children's affective and cognitive perspective-taking ability. The subjects were 5-year-old (N=64) and 7-year-old (N=64) children. In each group, there were equal number of boys and girls, 128 Children were composed of(male: 32, female: 32) and orphanage Children(male: 32, female: 32). Feshbach & Roe's child perspective-taking ability test was modified for this study and the test included four facial expression cards and six different stories including three types of affection. In the procedure of study, after a set of story cards containing one of three affective types, the subject was asked to retell the story shown in each cards to study cognitive perspective-taking ability, and for the affective perspective-taking ability. Then the subject was asked to tell the feeling of the hero in the story and to select one of the four facial expression which is consistent with the feeling. The cognitive perspective-taking ability and affective perspective-taking ability response were coded three kinds of scoring respectively. Scored data were analyzed with MANOVA. t-test, Pearson Correlations and Fisher-z test. The results were shown as follow : First, both Orpanage and Normal Children's the cognitive and affective perspective-taking ability increased with age. Secondly, both Orpanage and Normal Children's perspective-taking ability was lower than Normal Children's perspective-taking ability. Thirdly, both Orpanage and Normal Children's correlation between cognitive and affective perspective-taking ability increased with age.

  • PDF

Effects of Theory of Mind and Affective Perspective Taking on Young Children's Display Rule Behavior and Understanding (마음 이론과 감정조망수용능력이 유아의 표출 규칙 행동 및 이해에 미치는 영향)

  • Bae, Yun Jin;Choi, Bo Ga
    • Korean Journal of Child Studies
    • /
    • v.29 no.4
    • /
    • pp.65-77
    • /
    • 2008
  • This study investigated differences of display rule by age and gender and the effects of theory of mind and affective perspective taking on display rule. Subjects were 64 4- to 5-year old children. Instruments were false belief, appearance-reality distinction, affective perspective taking, gift-giving, and display rule understanding task. Findings were (1) Display rule understanding differed by age; older children understood the display rules better than younger children. (2) Theory of mind influenced positive display rule behavior. (3) Theory of mind and affective perspective taking had a significant effect on display rule understanding.

  • PDF

Differences between Institutionalized and Home-reared Children in Social Skills and Affective Perspective Taking (시설보호 아동과 일반아동의 사회적 기술 정서조망 능력의 비교)

  • Kwon, Se Eun;Yi, Soon Hyung
    • Korean Journal of Child Studies
    • /
    • v.23 no.2
    • /
    • pp.107-120
    • /
    • 2002
  • This study compared the social skills and affective perspective taking of institutionalized and home-reared children. The subjects were 59 institutionalized and 60 home-reared children in Seoul. Results showed that institutionalized children were lower in cooperation, self-assertion and self-control than home-reared children. Institutionalized children were lower in delight, sadness, fear, and anger than home-reared children. As skills in self-control increased, the level of affective perspective taking about fear increased.

  • PDF

The Relation between Perspective-taking Skills and Communication Abilities of Kindergarten Children (유아의 조망능력과 의사소통능력과의 관계)

  • Yu, Hui Chung
    • Korean Journal of Child Studies
    • /
    • v.9 no.1
    • /
    • pp.1-17
    • /
    • 1988
  • This study was designed to investigate the relation between perspective-taking skills and communication abilities of young children. The major purposes of this study were to study the relation between perspective-taking skills and communication abilities, to investigate the relation perspective-taking skills and communication abilities with differing listeners, to investigate sex differences in communication abilities within the higher group the perspective-taking skills with differing listeners, and to determine differences between the sexes in communication abilities within the higher group of perspective-taking skills in different situational conditions. Sixty kindergarteners (30 boys and 30 girls) were tested on their perspective taking abilities and divided into two groups, the higher and the lower groups. Five instruments were used in this study: spatial, affective and cognitive perspective-taking tests, expression-ability test, and a communication ability test. The collected data were statistically analyzed by using Pearson's r, point biserial coefficient correlations (rpb), t-test, and three-way analyses of variance with one factor repeated measurement. There were significant relations between spatial, cognitive perspective-taking skills and kindergartener's communication abilities. There were no significant differences in communication abilities in differing listeners and situational conditions. In the group whose level of perspective taking-skills was high, differences between the sexes were found in spatial, and affective perspective-taking skills, only in case of differing listeners.

  • PDF

The Effects of Age, Empathy, and Perspective Taking Ability on Altruistic Lying of Young Children (아동의 연령, 공감능력 및 조망수용능력이 이타적 거짓말에 미치는 영향)

  • Lee, Ji-Hye;Song, Hana
    • Korean Journal of Child Studies
    • /
    • v.35 no.4
    • /
    • pp.167-177
    • /
    • 2014
  • The study examined the influences of age, empathy and perspective taking ability on altruistic lying in 5 and 6 year old children. Eighty three children answered a question as to whether a protagonist would lie after listening to three vignettes involving altruistic lies. Korean versions of the Affective Situation Test(AST) and cognitive perspective taking task were used to measure children's empathy and perspective taking respectively. The results of the study showed that there were significant differences in altruistic lying by age. Altruistic lie by children was positively related with their age, empathy and perspective taking ability. In particular, age and perspective taking ability are important factors influencing children's altruistic lie in young children in Korea.

Effectiveness of the "Understanding Activities of Self and Others" Program in Improving Self-concept, Perspective-taking and Prosocial Behavior (자신과 타인이해 활동이 유아의 자아개념 조망수용 및 친사회적 행동에 미치는 영향)

  • Shin, Hyoun Ock;Kang, Moon Hee
    • Korean Journal of Child Studies
    • /
    • v.17 no.2
    • /
    • pp.121-139
    • /
    • 1996
  • The purpose of the present study was to explore a model program to improve preschool children's self-concept, perspective-taking and prosocial behavior through "Understanding Activities of Self and Others." Sixty 5- to 6-year-old children were selected by random sampling from a Kindergarten located in Seoul. Thirty children were in the experimental group and 30 were in the control group. The 8-week intervention program used the instrument, "Understanding Activities of Self and Others," constructed by the present researchers. One-way ANOVA and MANOVA were used for data analysis. At the end of the 8-week period, significant differences were found between the experimental and control groups in self-concept, perspective-taking ability, and prosocial behavior. This indicates the effectiveness of the present study in improving children's affective, cognitive, and behavioral traits.

  • PDF

The Influence of the Systematic Analogies Used at the Interpretation of Experimental Results on High School Students' Conceptual Change of Enzymes (실험 결과 해석 과정에서 사용한 체계적 비유가 고등학생들의 효소 개념 변화에 미친 영향)

  • Lee, Won-Kyung;Kim, Heui-Baik
    • Journal of The Korean Association For Science Education
    • /
    • v.27 no.7
    • /
    • pp.663-675
    • /
    • 2007
  • Chemical reactions in cells are so complicated and abstract that students have difficulty in understanding them. In this study, classes with the application of systematic analogies used at the interpretation of experimental results were taught to 10th-grade students in order to help them to understand the concept of enzymes, which play an important role in chemical reactions in cells. Effects of the classes on their understanding of the concept of enzymes and the role of systematic analogies were analyzed. The gap of understanding between the test group and the control group was significant at 0.05, indicating that systematic analogies are effective for students' understanding of the concept of enzymes. Looking into the concept of enzymes by individual element, the effect of systematic analogies was shown to be large for equilibrium-like processes, such as the enzyme structure change caused by temperature and pH; and the continuous and random actions of enzymes, which students have difficulty in understanding. For these processes, systematic analogies played a positive role in improving their conceptual status. The visualizations and familiarity of analogs increased their intelligibility regarding the concept of enzyme. Also, the systematic analogies increases their plausibility by helping to connect phenomena, taking place in the enzyme reaction experiments, with scientific concepts as scaffold. Accordingly, it was possible to explain experimental results as scientific concepts in a consistent manner. In addition, analogies familiar to students played a positive role from the affective perspective by promoting students' interest and helping them to approach hard scientific concepts.

Scientific Empathy Discovered in Scientists' Problem-Solving Process (과학자의 문제 해결 과정에서 탐색된 과학 공감)

  • Yang, Heesun;Kang, Seong-Joo
    • Journal of The Korean Association For Science Education
    • /
    • v.39 no.2
    • /
    • pp.249-261
    • /
    • 2019
  • The purpose of this study is to extract empathy factor in scientists' problem-solving process and to examine how the empathy factor influences scientists' problem-solving situation. In this study, we selected six common persons among the scientists mentioned by creativity researchers. And through their autobiographies and biographies, we extracted elements of empathy from their case of problem-solving and categorized them. We analyzed cases from 12 books and 50 papers using Davis' empathy scale as an analysis framework and extracted common factors. As a result, the scientific empathy elements were extracted from a total of 182 cases, and 33 common elements were found. The validity of this case was verified through the content validity test of the science education specialist group. As a result, the I-CVI average was .86 and the S-CVI average was .90. For the empathy elements that scientists used in problem-solving cases, in cognitive empathy, three elements (empathy through other disciplines, empathy from the perspective of the research object, accommodating others' opinions) were extracted in terms of perspective-taking, and three elements (imagination thought experiment based on observation, thought experiment, feeling like part of object) in fantasy. And in affective empathy, three elements (influenced by fellow researchers' motivation, touching from the subject, excitement studying more) were extracted in terms of empathic concern and two elements (heartache for others' failure in their research, sensitivity to problems) in personal depression. This could not be said to be a perfect match for Davis' empathy, but it would be possible to define the scientific empathy elements based on these common elements found in the scientists' cases.