• 제목/요약/키워드: phrase-initial

검색결과 44건 처리시간 0.027초

On Minimalist Requirements in Syntax

  • Lee, Hong-Bae
    • 한국영어학회지:영어학
    • /
    • 제3권2호
    • /
    • pp.255-280
    • /
    • 2003
  • The present paper will argue what can be considered to be principled elements of the initial state S/sub 0/ of the Faculty of Language, which are called the Interface Condition (IC), and how far we can take the strongest minimalist thesis (SMT), which aims to offer principled explanation of language in terms of IC and the principle of efficient computation, to linguistic analysis. We will discuss implications of label-free phrase structures, required by the strong version of the Inclusiveness Condition, and possibilities of crash-free syntax, required by the condition of efficient computation. I will point out problems of Chomsky's assumption that an externally Merged expletive there is a head, which, as a probe, undergoes agreement with the goal T. I will present several advantages we obtain if we maintain A and A' distinction, and assume that wh-movement to the outer [SPEC, υ] is an A'-movement like wh-movement to [SPEC, C].

  • PDF

The Role of Prosodic Boundary Cues in Word Segmentation in Korean

  • Kim, Sa-Hyang
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제13권1호
    • /
    • pp.29-41
    • /
    • 2006
  • This study investigates the degree to which various prosodic cues at the boundaries of prosodic phrases in Korean contribute to word segmentation. Since most phonological words in Korean are produced as one Accentual Phrase (AP), it was hypothesized that the detection of acoustic cues at AP boundaries would facilitate word segmentation. The prosodic characteristics of Korean APs include initial strengthening at the beginning of the phrase and pitch rise and final lengthening at the end. A perception experiment utilizing an artificial language learning paradigm revealed that cues conforming to the aforementioned prosodic characteristics of Korean facilitated listeners' word segmentation. Results also indicated that duration and amplitude cues were more helpful in segmentation than pitch. Nevertheless, results did show that a pitch cue that did not conform to the Korean AP interfered with segmentation.

  • PDF

한국어 특수조사 ‘-만’에 연계된 초점의 음향음성학적 특성 (The Acoustic Characteristics of Focus Associated with the Korean Particle' -man')

  • 최재응;전윤실;장윤;박순복;김기호
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제5권2호
    • /
    • pp.77-91
    • /
    • 1999
  • The purpose of this paper is to investigate the phonetic characteristics of the 'focus' phrases associated with the particle '-man' in Korean. The particle '-man' is a bound morpheme which, like other postpositions such as the subject marker '-ka' and the object marker '-lil', the so-called 'case markers' in Korean, typically attaches to a noun (phrase). The semantics of '-man' roughly corresponds to that of only, its counterpart in English, and is thus classified as a 'delimiter' (Yang 1973). It is assumed in this paper that '-man', like only in English, should have a 'focus' associated with it (von Stechow 1991, Rooth 1992). In general, '-man' attached phrases get the focus, but sometimes the association is not clear-cut, especially in the cases of emphatic use of '-man' or when the context strongly favors other phrase as the focus (Choe 1996). In this paper, we compare the phonetic characteristics of the '-man' marked phrases with those to which '-ka'/'-lil' is attached, and conclude that the focused '-man' phrases show higher fundamental frequencies than their equally focused 'case' -marked counterparts. However, when the context clearly forces the focus to fall on phrases other than the '-man' or '-ka'/'-lil' attached ones, there is no meaningful difference in fundamental frequency between the '-man' and '-ka'/'-lil' attached phrases. We also compare the phonetic characteristics of the regular use of '-man' with those of the emphatic '-man'. According to our experiments, the emphatic '-man' does not bring forth its phonetic effects, namely, higher fundamental frequencies, on the' -man' attached words or phrases but rather in various other ways such as higher fundamental frequencies in '-man', lengthening of the following word-initial syllable, or the inclusion of the following word in the same accentual phrase. Finally, it is claimed that '-man' associated focus phenomena, especially the emphatic use of '-man', show some typical acoustic characteristics of the other well-known focus phenomena, namely, wh-interrogatives.

  • PDF

영어의 강음절(강세 음절)과 한국어 화자의 단어 분절 (Strong (stressed) syllables in English and lexical segmentation by Koreans)

  • 김선미;남기춘
    • 말소리와 음성과학
    • /
    • 제3권1호
    • /
    • pp.3-14
    • /
    • 2011
  • It has been posited that in English, native listeners use the Metrical Segmentation Strategy (MSS) for the segmentation of continuous speech. Strong syllables tend to be perceived as potential word onsets for English native speakers, which is due to the high proportion of strong syllables word-initially in the English vocabulary. This study investigates whether Koreans employ the same strategy when segmenting speech input in English. Word-spotting experiments were conducted using vowel-initial and consonant-initial bisyllabic targets embedded in nonsense trisyllables in Experiment 1 and 2, respectively. The effect of strong syllable was significant in the RT (reaction times) analysis but not in the error analysis. In both experiments, Korean listeners detected words more slowly when the word-initial syllable is strong (stressed) than when it is weak (unstressed). However, the error analysis showed that there was no effect of initial stress in Experiment 1 and in the item (F2) analysis in Experiment 2. Only the subject (F1) analysis in Experiment 2 showed that the participants made more errors when the word starts with a strong syllable. These findings suggest that Koran listeners do not use the Metrical Segmentation Strategy for segmenting English speech. They do not treat strong syllables as word beginnings, but rather have difficulties recognizing words when the word starts with a strong syllable. These results are discussed in terms of intonational properties of Korean prosodic phrases which are found to serve as lexical segmentation cues in the Korean language.

  • PDF

한국인의 영어 어두 유성파열음의 습득과 후속모음 높이와의 관계 (Acquisition of English Voiced Stop in Word Initial Position: Correlation with Vowel Height)

  • 윤수연;서민경;송윤경
    • 한국음향학회:학술대회논문집
    • /
    • 한국음향학회 2000년도 하계학술발표대회 논문집 제19권 1호
    • /
    • pp.321-324
    • /
    • 2000
  • 한국어는 어두에서 유성파열음이 나타나지 않고 약한 기식이 있는 연자음으로 실현되므로 영어의 유성파열음을 발음하기 어렵다. 한국인이 어두 유성파열음을 습득할 때 후속하는 모음의 높이가 영향을 미치리라 가정하고 /이, 에, 어, 우/ 4개의 모음을 선택하여 어두유성파열음 의 VOT에 영향을 미치는가를 단독단어와 문장에서 살펴보았다. native, 숙련자, 미숙련자의 세 그룹으로 나누어 실시한 결과 native의 경우 후속하는 모음이 어두파열음의 VOT에 영향을 끼치지 않았으며, 이러한 경향은 숙련자 그룹에서도 지켜짐을 알 수 있었다. 그러나 미숙련자 그룹인 경우 고모음이 저모음에서보다 VOT가 현저하게 길었고 통계검사 결과 유의미한 차이를 보여, 고모음에서 유성발음을 잘 못함을 알 수 있었다. native와 숙련자 그룹은 intermediate phrase(이하 iP) initial인 단어나 iP medial인 문장에서 VOT가 거의 유사한데 비해 미숙련자 그룹에서는 문장 내에서 VOT가 조금씩 짧아졌고 이 경향은 고모음에서 두드러져 통계적으로 유의미한 차이를 보였다.

  • PDF

How does focus-induced prominence modulate phonetic realizations for Korean word-medial stops?

  • Choi, Jiyoun
    • 말소리와 음성과학
    • /
    • 제12권4호
    • /
    • pp.57-61
    • /
    • 2020
  • Previous research has indicated that the patterns of phonetic modulations induced by prominence are not consistent across languages but are conditioned by sound systems specific to a given language. Most studies examining the prominence effects in Korean have been restricted to segments in word-initial and phrase-initial positions. The present study, thus, set out to explore the prominence effects for Korean stop consonants in word-medial intervocalic positions. A total of 16 speakers of Seoul Korean (8 males, 8 females) produced word-medial intervocalic lenis and aspirated stops with and without prominence. The prominence was induced by contrast focus on the phonation-type contrast, that is, lenis vs. aspirated stops. Our results showed that F0 of vowels following both lenis and aspirated stops became higher when the target stops received focus than when they did not, whereas voice onset time (VOT) and voicing during stop closure for both lenis and aspirated stops did not differ between the focus and no-focus conditions. The findings add to our understanding of diverse patterns of prominence-induced strengthening on the acoustic realizations of segments.

우리말 동철이음어(同綴異音語) IPA.로마자 표기 (사~섬) (Heteronyms in modern Korean and their transcription in the IPA and the Roman alphabet)

  • 유만근
    • 대한음성학회지:말소리
    • /
    • 제37호
    • /
    • pp.49-71
    • /
    • 1999
  • The Purpose of this paper is to gather pairs of heteronyms in modern Korean and transcribe them in the IPA and the Roman alphabet in order to propose that all of them should be differentiated in Hanngul orthography. More than a quarter of the whole Korean vocabulary consists of words with a long vowel and the number of minimal pairs distinguished only by the chroneme reaches nearly ten thousand (i.e. twenty thousand words). The letter h syllable-finally is used here to represent the long vowel in Romanization except the vowel '으‘[?:] which is transcribed by doubling the letter u (i.e. uu). Another factor bringing forth lots of heteronyms in Korean is the lack of full indication as to the non-automatic reinforcement in the initial consonant of a word (or a morpheme) when preceded by another within a phrase (or a word). These reinforced word-initial consonants are written with the letter c and an apostrophe (like c'g- , c'd- , c'b-, c's-, c'j-) in Romanization here. The reinforced morpheme-initial consonant within a word is written with the letters k t, p, ss and cz for ㄲ, ㄸ, ㅃ, ㅆ and ㅉ sounds respectively. The contrasted pronunciations of pairs of heteronyms beginning with ㅅ /s/sup h// and ㅆ /s/ sounds are transcribed here for exemplification.

  • PDF

우리말 동철이음어 구별표기안 - IPA, 로마자, 한글표기를 나란히 견주어 -

  • 유만근
    • 대한음성학회지:말소리
    • /
    • 제31_32호
    • /
    • pp.51-82
    • /
    • 1996
  • The purpose of this paper is to gather pairs of heteronyms in Modem Korean and to propose that all of them should be differentiated in both the Hanngul orthography and Romanization as well as in the IPA transcription. More than a quarter of the whole Korean vocabulary consists of words with a long vowel and the number of minimal pairs distinguished only by the chroneme reaches nearly ten thousand (ie. twenty thousand words). It is suggested here that the letter s in Hanngul and the letter 'h' in the Roman alphabet be used to represent the long vowel. Another factor which brings forth lots of heteronyms in Korean is the lacking of enough indication as to non-automatic reinforcement in the initial consonant o( a word (or a morpheme) when following another within a phrase (or a word). It is proposed here that the non-automatincally rienforced word-initial consonant should be written with the letter h (like ㅺ, ㅼ, ㅽ, ㅾ) and an apostrophe (like 물'새 or 밭'이랑, 물'약) in Hanngul, and with the letter c and an apostrophe (like c'g-, c'd-, c'b-, c'j- ) in the Roman alphabet The morpheme-initial reinforced consonant within a word is written with the letters k, 1, p and cz for ㅺ, ㅼ, ㅽ, and ㅾ respectively. The contrasted pronunciations of pairs of heteronyms beginning with ㅁ/m sound are transcribed here for exemplification in the IPA, Roman alphabet and Hanngul.

  • PDF

Intonational Characteristics of Korean Focus Realization by American Learners of Korean

  • Oh, Mi-Ra;Kang, Sun-Mi;Kim, Kee-Ho
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제11권1호
    • /
    • pp.131-145
    • /
    • 2004
  • The informative or important entities in utterances are focused and the focused items are usually accompanied by changes in phonetic manifestation. Phonetic realizations triggered by focus include changes of tonal contours as well as segmental strengthening. Focus in Korean is characterized by new phrase initiation, dephrasing, and initial tone contour with an enlarged pitch range in addition to segmentally lengthened initial segment. Focusing on the prosodic cues which play an important role in delivering the speakers' intention, this study aims to find out what intonational characteristics of Korean focus are realized by English learners of Korean. The English learners are divided into two groups according to their fluency in Korean, and the differences in focus realization between each group are discussed. Furthermore, the phonological and phonetic realizations of focus by English learners of Korean are compared to those by Korean native speakers. The results of this study yields two suggestions for Korean intonation education of L2 learners. First, the comparison between the two speaker groups can give better understanding in how and why the Korean intonation of English speakers is different from that of Koreans. Second, each phonological and phonetic characteristic of focus realization can weigh differently and its realization provides a criterion for evaluation of L2 Korean proficiency.

  • PDF

Linguistic and social factors affecting the /ɨ/ and /ʌ/ dispersion in Kyungsang Korean

  • Choe, Wook Kyung;Lee, Dongmyung
    • 말소리와 음성과학
    • /
    • 제9권4호
    • /
    • pp.69-76
    • /
    • 2017
  • The current study investigated the productions of /ɨ/ and /${\Lambda}$/ in Kyungsang Korean, which is known for undergoing a dispersion for the younger generation. Specifically, to identify the nature of /ɨ/ and /${\Lambda}$/ in Kyungsang Korean, this study examined the linguistic and social factors affecting directions and degrees of the /ɨ/-/${\Lambda}$/ dispersion. Sixteen young speakers of Kyungsang Korean repeated 112 (near) minimal pairs containing the two target vowels. The formant values of each production as well as the Euclidean distance between the two vowels were analyzed for four manipulated factors: gender (male vs. female), the existence of carrier phrases (words in isolation vs. words with a carrier phrase), the lexical status of stimulus words (real-word pairs vs. nonsense-word pairs), and the vowel position within a word (word-initial positions vs. word-final positions). The results indicated that the female speakers produced the two target vowels more distinctively than the male speakers, and so did when the words were produced in isolation. The results also revealed that the Euclidean distances were greater for the real-word pairs and in word-initial positions. Overall, the results suggested that the Kyungsang Korean speakers in their 20s could distinctively produce the two vowels /ɨ/ and /${\Lambda}$/, but this vowel dispersion is not a completed process, but an ongoing one.