• 제목/요약/키워드: accusative case markers

검색결과 10건 처리시간 0.023초

Case Variation in Guarani

  • Yang, Jeong-Seok
    • 한국언어정보학회지:언어와정보
    • /
    • 제14권1호
    • /
    • pp.93-111
    • /
    • 2010
  • This article is a description of the case variation in Guarani Language, which is a relatively, rarely studied language, and more so about case phenomena. Guarani has two remarkable facts about case. First, it has two overt accusative case markers, which are differentiated by the semantic notion of boundedness as in Jackendoff(1990, 1991). The existence of accusative case markers in Guarani is attested by their behavior in the typical transitive verb sentences, the ability to occur in ECM constructions, and the interpretation of specificity which is parallel to Turkish accusative case marker realization reported in Enc(1991). Second, accusative case forms occur in adjunct positions as well as object positions in Guarani. To capture these peculiar case phenomena, an account based on some recent Minimalist ideas about case checking from Legate(2008), Bowers(2010) is shown to be available.

  • PDF

한(韓)·일(日)·유(琉) 격조사 비교연구 - 주격(主格)·목적격(目的格) 조사를 중심으로 - (A Comparative Study of Case Markers in Korean, Japanese and Ryukyuan Languages: Focusing on Nominative Case Markers and Accusative Case Markers)

  • 이갑
    • 비교문화연구
    • /
    • 제46권
    • /
    • pp.355-377
    • /
    • 2017
  • 한국어(韓國語) 일본어(日本語) 유구어(琉球語) 세 언어는 여러 면에서 비슷한 점이 있다. 본 논문은 한국어(韓國語) 일본어(日本語) 유구어(琉球語) 세 언어의 문법 범주인 격조사를 연구 대상으로 삼고주격조사와 목적격조사를 중심으로 하여 격조사의 형태와 기능을 분석하였다. 이를 바탕으로 하여 세 언어의 유사성과 차이점을 밝히고 앞으로 더 심층적인 연구에 기초 자료를 제공하고자 하였다. 이 연구를 통해 한국어(韓國語) 일본어(日本語) 유구어(琉球語)의 주격조사와 목적격조사는 중세국어시기부터 이미 상당한 형태상의 차이가 남을 밝혔다. 형태 차이에도 불구하고 세 언어의 주격조사와 목적격조사는 기능면에서 유사한 점이 많다. 주격조사와 목적격조사가 다른 조사로 나타나는 경우 세 언어는 각각 차이점을 보였다. 한국어의 주격조사와 목적격조사는 다 보조사로 쓰일 수 있으며, 일본어의 경우는 주격조사가 접속조사로 사용될 수 있고 목적격조사는 간투조사(間投助詞)로 사용될 수 있다. 유구어의 주격조사는 일본어의 주격조사와 유사한 용법으로 사용된다.

중국 조선족 아동들의 한국어 문법 기능 습득 (Acquisition of Grammatical Functions of the Korean Language by Korean-Chinese Children : A Short-Term Longitudinal Study)

  • 이귀옥;김형재;이혜련
    • 아동학회지
    • /
    • 제27권2호
    • /
    • pp.1-14
    • /
    • 2006
  • This study examined the spontaneous utterances of 20 1-2 year old Korean-Chinese children in Yanji, China by videotaping their spontaneous natural speech during interaction with his/her caregiver for 30 minutes on 4 separate occasions. Utterances were transcribed and coded by nominative and accusative case markers; then analyzed by grammatical functions. Results showed that the order and pattern of the acquisition of the case marking system of Korean-Chinese bilinguals differed from that of Korean monolinguals. Bilinguals used the accusative marker -lul earlier than the nominative marker-ka and overextended the accusative marker more often than the nominative marker. These results are consistent with those of Gathercole (in press) who found that bilingual children differed from monolinguals in mastering grammar.

  • PDF

$30{\sim}47$ 개월 일반아동의 의문사 질문 이해 발달: 누가, 누구를, 누구한테, 무엇이, 무엇을 (The Comprehension of 'who' and 'what' Questions in Normally Developing Korean Children)

  • 정미란;황민아
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제13권3호
    • /
    • pp.207-219
    • /
    • 2006
  • The present study was designed to investigate the comprehension of 'who' and 'what' questions in 2- to 3-year-old normal children. Sixty children were divided into 3 groups depending on their ages, i.e., age groups 2;6-2;11, 3;0-3:5, and 3;6-3;11. Three types of 'who' questions and 2 types of 'what' questions were generated depending on the attached case markers, i.e., who-nominative, who-accusative, who-dative, what-nominative, and what-accusative. The children watched 36 cuts of short video recordings. After watching each cut, they were asked to answer one of the 5 types of wh-questions. For the 'who-nominative' and 'what-accusative' questions, even the late 2-year-old children performed with over 70% of accuracy, and the late 3-year-old children performed with over 95% of accuracy. For the 'who-accusative' and 'who-dative' questions, the late 2-year olds exhibited difficulty in comprehension with performance accuracy of 41% and 33%, respectively. However, the late 3-year olds could comprehend those questions correctly with over 90% of accuracy. On the other hand, in answering 'what-nominative' questions, the children did not show rapid development across the age groups, as the mean performance accuracies of the 3 groups were 39%, 49%, and 59%, respectively. The results indicated that children's understanding of a wh- question is largely affected by the case of the interrogative.

  • PDF

경계선지능 언어발달장애아동과 일반아동의 문법성 판단 및 오류수정 - 조사를 중심으로 - (Grammaticality Judgement and Error Correction by Children with Developmental Language Impairments)

  • 임종아;황민아
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제13권2호
    • /
    • pp.59-72
    • /
    • 2006
  • In the present study, we investigated the grammaticality judgement skills of children with developmental language impairments. The participants included 20 children with language impairments of ages ranging from 7 to 9 years and of IQ's ranging from 71 to 84, and 40 normally developing children. Twenty normal children were matched with the language impaired children in their language ages and the other 20 normal children were matched with the language impaired children in their chronological ages. The children were asked to judge the grammatical correctness of 48 short sentences, half of which were ungrammatical sentences containing incorrect case-markers and the other half were grammatically correct sentences. Four types of case-markers including nominative "i/ga", accusative "ul/lul", locative "e," and instrumental "ro" were systematically changed to generate the ungrammatical sentences. The language impaired children performed worse than both groups of normally developing children in detecting the ungrammatical sentences and in correcting the case-markers of those sentences. In detecting the errors of ungrammatical sentences, the language impaired children exhibited variable performances across the different case-markers.

  • PDF

Case Drop and Prosodic Structure in Korean

  • Hong, Sung-Hoon
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제7권3호
    • /
    • pp.35-51
    • /
    • 2000
  • The goal of this paper is to examine how Case Drop (the drop of the case markers) correlates with the prosodic structure in Korean. On the assumption that intervocalic Lenis Stop Voicing (LSV) applies within the domain of the Accentual Phrase (AP), voicing analyses are performed on intervocalic lenis stop consonants before and after Case Drop. A statistical analysis reveals that the drop of the nominative and accusative case markers significantly alter the AP structure. Pitch values will then be extracted to verify that such changes in the AP structure conform to the pitch properties proposed for the AP (Jun 1993, 1998). The results show that the AP structure suggested by LSV does not always coincide with that imposed by the pitch properties.

  • PDF

말 늦은 아동의 문장 이해 전략 (Sentence interpretation strategies by typically developing and late-talking Korean toddlers)

  • 조수정;황민아;최경순
    • 말소리와 음성과학
    • /
    • 제6권3호
    • /
    • pp.13-21
    • /
    • 2014
  • Late talkers are young children who are delayed in their expressive language skills despite normal nonverbal cognitive ability, adequate hearing and typical personality development. The purpose of this study is to investigate the sentence interpretation strategies used by Korean-speaking late talkers and age-matched normal children. Nine late talkers and nine normal children matched by age at 30-35months were participated in this study. 27 simple noun-noun-verb(NNV) sentences were generated by factorial combination of case-marker [nominal case-marker on the first noun and accusative on the second (C1), accusative on the first noun and nominative on the second (C2), and no case markers on both nouns (C0)], and animacy of the nouns [animate-inanimate(AI), inanimate-animate(IA), animate-animate(AA)]. All the children were asked to "act out" their interpretation of the given sentence. For each type of sentences the percentage of choices of the first noun as the agent was calculated. The results of group (2) ${\times}$ animacy(3) ${\times}$ case-marker(3) mixed ANOVA showed a significant main effect for 'animacy', 'case marker' and 'group(2) ${\times}$ case-marker (3)'. The late talkers relied on semantic (animacy) cues in their interpretation of the sentences, while the normal peers utilized both animacy and grammatical morpheme (case-marker) cues. The results indicated that the late-talkers' comprehension skills were also delayed.

A Syntactic Account of the Properties of Bare Nominals in Discourse

  • Ahn, Hee-Don;Cho, Sung-Eun
    • 한국언어정보학회:학술대회논문집
    • /
    • 한국언어정보학회 2007년도 정기학술대회
    • /
    • pp.57-66
    • /
    • 2007
  • Case markers in Korean are omissible in colloquial speech. Previous discourse studies of Caseless bare NPs in Korean show that the information structure of zero Nominative not only differs from that of overt Nominative but it also differs from that of zero Accusative in many respects. This paper aims to provide a basis for these semantic/pragmatic properties of Caseless NPs through the syntactic difference between bare subjects and bare objects: namely, the former are left-dislocated NPs, whereas the latter form complex predicates with the subcategorizing verbs. Our analysis will account for the facts that (i) the distribution of bare subject NPs are more restricted than that of bare object NPs; (ii) bare subject NPs must be specific or topical; (iii) Acc-marked NPs in canonical position tend to be focalized.

  • PDF

Focus Types and Gradients in Korean Case Ellipsis

  • Lee, Han-Jung
    • 한국언어정보학회지:언어와정보
    • /
    • 제12권2호
    • /
    • pp.1-20
    • /
    • 2008
  • This paper examines the effects of focus types on case ellipsis in Korean. A number of previous studies have suggested that accusative case markers in Korean and Japanese cannot be dropped when the object they mark is contrastively focused (Masunaga, 1988; Yatabe, 1999; Ko, 2000; Lee, 2002). Using experimental evidence, we argue against the view that case ellipsis in Korean is sensitive to the distinction between contrastive vs. non-contrastive focus. An alternative analysis is proposed which accounts for the phenomenon of variable case marking in terms of the interaction between the contrastive strength and the discourse accessibility of focused object NPs. By viewing patterns of case ellipsis as the result of balancing between these two forces, such an analysis can correctly predict the gradient pattern of case ellipsis shown by the three types of focused objects tested in the experiment (contrastive replacing focus, contrastive selecting focus and non-contrastive, informational focus), while at the same time offering an explanation for why subtypes of focus exert distinct influences on case ellipsis.

  • PDF

Subject-Object Asymmetries of Morphological Case Realization

  • Ahn, Hee-Don;Cho, Sung-Eun
    • 한국언어정보학회지:언어와정보
    • /
    • 제11권1호
    • /
    • pp.53-76
    • /
    • 2007
  • Case markers in Korean are omissible in colloquial speech. Previous discourse studies of Caseless bare NPs in Korean show that the information structure of zero Nominative not only differs from that of overt Nominative but it also differs from that of zero Accusative in many respects. This paper aims to provide a basis for these semantic/pragmatic properties of Caseless NPs through the syntactic difference between bare subjects and bare objects: namely, the former are left-dislocated NPs, whereas the latter form complex predicates with the subcategorizing verbs. Our analysis will account for the facts that (i) the distribution of bare subject NPs are more restricted than that of bare object NPs; (ii) bare subject NPs must be specific or topical; (iii) Acc-marked NPs in canonical position tend to be focalized.

  • PDF