Browse > Article
http://dx.doi.org/10.13064/KSSS.2021.13.3.053

Perception of Japanese word-initial stops by native listeners  

Byun, Hi-Gyung (Akita International University)
Publication Information
Phonetics and Speech Sciences / v.13, no.3, 2021 , pp. 53-64 More about this Journal
Abstract
It is known that the voicing contrast for Japanese word-initial stops is primarily realized as differences in the voice onset time (VOT). However, recent studies have reported that voiced stops are more often produced with a positive VOT than with a negative VOT among the younger generation nationwide. It is also known that post-stop F0 is associated with the stop contrast, but the degree of F0 use differs from region to region. This study explores whether the difference in post-stop F0 functions as a perceptual cue to the stop contrast along with VOT. Fifty-five college students who are native listeners from four different regions participated in two or three perception tests. The results show that VOT is a primary cue to the voiced-voiceless distinction of word-initial stops, but that the effect of post-stop F0 on the stop contrast is marginal. The post-stop F0 is involved in perception only when VOT is ambiguous, such that a sound with high F0 is more often perceived as a voiceless stop, but not vice versa. The results of this study indicate that the acoustic parameters associated with the stop contrast are not the same in production and perception, and suggest that other factors such as context, which is not an acoustic characteristic, may also be involved in the stop contrast.
Keywords
Japanese; word-initial stop; perceptual cue; VOT; post-stop F0;
Citations & Related Records
Times Cited By KSCI : 1  (Citation Analysis)
연도 인용수 순위
1 Lee, H., Politzer-Ahles, S., & Jongman, A. (2013). Speakers of tonal and non-tonal Korean dialects use different cue weightings in the perception of the three-way laryngeal stop contrast. Journal of Phonetics, 41(2), 117-132.   DOI
2 Shimizu, K. (1999). Nichieigo niokeru heisashiin no yuseisei-museisei no onseiteki tokucho [A study on phonetic characteristics of voicing of stop consonants in Japanese and English]. Onsei Kenkyu, 3(2), 4-10.
3 Kong, E. J., Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2012). Voice onset time is necessary but not always sufficient to describe acquisition of voiced stops: The cases of Greek and Japanese. Journal of Phonetics, 40(6), 725-744.   DOI
4 Kim, M. (2004, October). Correlation between VOT and F0 in the perception of Korean stops and affricates. Proceedings of the Interspeech 2004 (pp. 49-52). Jeju, Korea.
5 Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1970, January). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: Cross-language tests. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 569-573). Prague, Czech.
6 Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1985). Relative power of cues: F0 shift versus voice timing. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Phonetic linguistics: Essays in honor of Peter Ladefoged (pp. 25-33). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
7 Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1973). Voice-timing perception in Spanish word-initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 1(1), 1-8.   DOI
8 Byun, H. G. (2021). Acoustic characteristics for Japanese stops in word-initial position: VOT and post-stop fo. Onsei Kenkyu, 25, 41-63.
9 Homma, Y. (1980). Voice onset time in Japanese stops. Onsei Gakkai Kaiho, 163, 7-9.
10 Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384-422.   DOI
11 Winn, M. B., Chatterjee, M., & Idsardi, W. J. (2013). Roles of voice onset time and F0 in stop consonant voicing perception: Effects of masking noise and low-pass filtering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1097-1107.   DOI
12 Otomo S., Sato, T., & Takakura, M. (1957). Nihongono musei haretsuon to yusei haretsuon no soi [On the difference between voiceless stop consonants and voiced stop consonants in Japanese sounds]. The bulletin of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 95, 4-9.
13 Shimizu, K. (1996). A cross-language study of voicing contrasts of stop consonants in Asian languages. Tokyo, Japan: Seibido.
14 Takada, M. (2006). Goto yusei haretsuon niokeru VOT no chi'ikisa to sedaisa: Tohoku kara Kanto no bunseki [Regional and generational variations of VOT in initial voiced plosives: An analysis of the Tohoku-Kanto area]. Nihongo no Kenkyu, 2(2), 34-45.
15 Kang, J., & Kong, E. (2014). The role of f0 cue in distinguishing a laryngeal contrast of word-initial stops in Korean and Japanese. Language and Linguistics, 62, 75-100.
16 Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice onset time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 75-86.   DOI
17 Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 6.0.22) [Computer program]. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/
18 Gao, J., & Arai, T. (2019). Plosive (de-)voicing and f0 perturbations in Tokyo Japanese: Positional variation, cue enhancement, and contrast recovery. Journal of Phonetics, 77, 100932.   DOI
19 Kobayashi, T. (1981). Nihongo no goto haretsuon no VOT [VOT in Japanese word-initial stops]. Gengo Bunka Kenkyujo, 7, 149-157.
20 Kurosaki, N. (2002). Boin ni zensetsusuru hatsuon nitsuite [On the syllable-final nasal followed by a vowel]. Kanagawa University Studies in Language, 25, 11-22.
21 Matsui, M. (2014, September). Hensokutekina goseion'o mochiita shiin yuseisei no chikakuni kansuru tegakari nitsusite [Perceptual cues to stop voicing with anomalous synthetic sounds]. Proceedings of the 28th General Meeting of the Phonetic Society of Japan (pp. 117-122). Tokyo, Japan.
22 Gao, J., Yun, J., & Arai, T. (2019, August). VOT-F0 coarticulation in Japanese: Production-biased or misparsing? Proceedings of the 19th ICPhS (pp. 210-214). Melbourne, Australia.
23 Itoh, M., Sasanuma, S., Tatsumi, I. F., & Kobayashi, Y. (1979). Voice onset time characteristics of apraxia of speech. Brain and Language, 17(2), 193-210.   DOI
24 Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1970, January). The voicing dimension: Some experiments on comparative phonetics. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 563-567). Prague, Czechoslovakia.
25 Riney, T. J., Takagi, N., Ota, K., & Uchida, Y. (2007). The intermediate degree of VOT in Japanese initial voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 35(3), 439-443.   DOI
26 Sato, T. (1958). On the differences in time structures of voiced and unvoiced stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan, 14, 117-122.   DOI
27 Kong, E. J. (2012). Perception of Korean stops with a three-way laryngeal contrast. Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 4(1), 13-20.   DOI
28 Takada, M., Kong, E. J., Yoneyama, K., & Beckman, M. E. (2016, October). Do pitch and voice quality cue word-initial "voicing" in Tohoku Japanese? The 24th Japanese/Korean Linguistics Conference. Tachikawa, Japan.
29 Takada, M. (2004). Nihongo no goto no yusei shikei haretsuon /d/ niokeru +VOTka to sedaisa [The +VOT tendency in initial alveolar plosive /d/ in Japanese and speakers' age]. Onsei Kenkyu, 8(3), 57-66.
30 Takada, M. (2011). Nihongo goto heisaon no kenkyu: VOT no kyo jiteki bunpu to tsujiteki henka [A study on the word-initial stops of Japanese: Synchronic distribution and diachronic change in VOT]. Tokyo, Japan: Kurosio.
31 Whalen, D. H., Abramson, A. S., Lisker, L., & Mody, M. (1990). Gradient effects of fundamental frequency on stop consonant voicing judgments. Phonetica, 47(1-2), 36-49.   DOI
32 Whalen, D. H., Abramson, A. S., Lisker, L., & Mody, M. (1993). F0 gives voicing information even with unambiguous voice onset times. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93(4), 2152-2159.   DOI