1 |
Lee, H., Politzer-Ahles, S., & Jongman, A. (2013). Speakers of tonal and non-tonal Korean dialects use different cue weightings in the perception of the three-way laryngeal stop contrast. Journal of Phonetics, 41(2), 117-132.
DOI
|
2 |
Shimizu, K. (1999). Nichieigo niokeru heisashiin no yuseisei-museisei no onseiteki tokucho [A study on phonetic characteristics of voicing of stop consonants in Japanese and English]. Onsei Kenkyu, 3(2), 4-10.
|
3 |
Kong, E. J., Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2012). Voice onset time is necessary but not always sufficient to describe acquisition of voiced stops: The cases of Greek and Japanese. Journal of Phonetics, 40(6), 725-744.
DOI
|
4 |
Kim, M. (2004, October). Correlation between VOT and F0 in the perception of Korean stops and affricates. Proceedings of the Interspeech 2004 (pp. 49-52). Jeju, Korea.
|
5 |
Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1970, January). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: Cross-language tests. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 569-573). Prague, Czech.
|
6 |
Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1985). Relative power of cues: F0 shift versus voice timing. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Phonetic linguistics: Essays in honor of Peter Ladefoged (pp. 25-33). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
|
7 |
Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1973). Voice-timing perception in Spanish word-initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 1(1), 1-8.
DOI
|
8 |
Byun, H. G. (2021). Acoustic characteristics for Japanese stops in word-initial position: VOT and post-stop fo. Onsei Kenkyu, 25, 41-63.
|
9 |
Homma, Y. (1980). Voice onset time in Japanese stops. Onsei Gakkai Kaiho, 163, 7-9.
|
10 |
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384-422.
DOI
|
11 |
Winn, M. B., Chatterjee, M., & Idsardi, W. J. (2013). Roles of voice onset time and F0 in stop consonant voicing perception: Effects of masking noise and low-pass filtering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1097-1107.
DOI
|
12 |
Otomo S., Sato, T., & Takakura, M. (1957). Nihongono musei haretsuon to yusei haretsuon no soi [On the difference between voiceless stop consonants and voiced stop consonants in Japanese sounds]. The bulletin of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 95, 4-9.
|
13 |
Shimizu, K. (1996). A cross-language study of voicing contrasts of stop consonants in Asian languages. Tokyo, Japan: Seibido.
|
14 |
Takada, M. (2006). Goto yusei haretsuon niokeru VOT no chi'ikisa to sedaisa: Tohoku kara Kanto no bunseki [Regional and generational variations of VOT in initial voiced plosives: An analysis of the Tohoku-Kanto area]. Nihongo no Kenkyu, 2(2), 34-45.
|
15 |
Kang, J., & Kong, E. (2014). The role of f0 cue in distinguishing a laryngeal contrast of word-initial stops in Korean and Japanese. Language and Linguistics, 62, 75-100.
|
16 |
Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice onset time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 75-86.
DOI
|
17 |
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 6.0.22) [Computer program]. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/
|
18 |
Gao, J., & Arai, T. (2019). Plosive (de-)voicing and f0 perturbations in Tokyo Japanese: Positional variation, cue enhancement, and contrast recovery. Journal of Phonetics, 77, 100932.
DOI
|
19 |
Kobayashi, T. (1981). Nihongo no goto haretsuon no VOT [VOT in Japanese word-initial stops]. Gengo Bunka Kenkyujo, 7, 149-157.
|
20 |
Kurosaki, N. (2002). Boin ni zensetsusuru hatsuon nitsuite [On the syllable-final nasal followed by a vowel]. Kanagawa University Studies in Language, 25, 11-22.
|
21 |
Matsui, M. (2014, September). Hensokutekina goseion'o mochiita shiin yuseisei no chikakuni kansuru tegakari nitsusite [Perceptual cues to stop voicing with anomalous synthetic sounds]. Proceedings of the 28th General Meeting of the Phonetic Society of Japan (pp. 117-122). Tokyo, Japan.
|
22 |
Gao, J., Yun, J., & Arai, T. (2019, August). VOT-F0 coarticulation in Japanese: Production-biased or misparsing? Proceedings of the 19th ICPhS (pp. 210-214). Melbourne, Australia.
|
23 |
Itoh, M., Sasanuma, S., Tatsumi, I. F., & Kobayashi, Y. (1979). Voice onset time characteristics of apraxia of speech. Brain and Language, 17(2), 193-210.
DOI
|
24 |
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1970, January). The voicing dimension: Some experiments on comparative phonetics. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 563-567). Prague, Czechoslovakia.
|
25 |
Riney, T. J., Takagi, N., Ota, K., & Uchida, Y. (2007). The intermediate degree of VOT in Japanese initial voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 35(3), 439-443.
DOI
|
26 |
Sato, T. (1958). On the differences in time structures of voiced and unvoiced stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan, 14, 117-122.
DOI
|
27 |
Kong, E. J. (2012). Perception of Korean stops with a three-way laryngeal contrast. Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 4(1), 13-20.
DOI
|
28 |
Takada, M., Kong, E. J., Yoneyama, K., & Beckman, M. E. (2016, October). Do pitch and voice quality cue word-initial "voicing" in Tohoku Japanese? The 24th Japanese/Korean Linguistics Conference. Tachikawa, Japan.
|
29 |
Takada, M. (2004). Nihongo no goto no yusei shikei haretsuon /d/ niokeru +VOTka to sedaisa [The +VOT tendency in initial alveolar plosive /d/ in Japanese and speakers' age]. Onsei Kenkyu, 8(3), 57-66.
|
30 |
Takada, M. (2011). Nihongo goto heisaon no kenkyu: VOT no kyo jiteki bunpu to tsujiteki henka [A study on the word-initial stops of Japanese: Synchronic distribution and diachronic change in VOT]. Tokyo, Japan: Kurosio.
|
31 |
Whalen, D. H., Abramson, A. S., Lisker, L., & Mody, M. (1990). Gradient effects of fundamental frequency on stop consonant voicing judgments. Phonetica, 47(1-2), 36-49.
DOI
|
32 |
Whalen, D. H., Abramson, A. S., Lisker, L., & Mody, M. (1993). F0 gives voicing information even with unambiguous voice onset times. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93(4), 2152-2159.
DOI
|