Browse > Article
http://dx.doi.org/10.5723/KJCS.2013.34.2.1

The Relationships among Infants' Nonverbal Communication, Maternal Verbal Behaviors and the Infants' Acquisition of Vocabulary  

Lee, Yun Seon (Child and Family Studies, Yonsei University)
Kim, Myoung Soon (Child and Family Studies, Yonsei University)
Publication Information
Korean Journal of Child Studies / v.34, no.2, 2013 , pp. 1-25 More about this Journal
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship among infants' nonverbal communication, maternal verbal behaviors and the infants' acquisition of vocabulary. The subjects were 93 pairs of 10 to 18 months old infants and their mothers residing in the Seoul and GyeongGi-Do area. The results were as follows : (1) In terms of the infants' nonverbal communicative means, it appeared that the group of 16~18 month olds utilized more gesture and vocalization. As regards the infants' nonverbal communicative functions in terms of social behavior and joint attention, the group of 16~18 month olds was found to perform more of the behaviors from this category than the other groups. There was a significant difference in the maternal verbal behavior among the different age groups. (2) Among the infants' nonverbal communicative means, gesture and vocalization, there appeared to be a significant relationship between vocalization and the infants' acquisition of vocabulary. In addition, there was an important relationship between the high usage of infants' nonverbal communicative functions with behavior regulation and the high usage of joint attention and the successful acquisition of vocabulary among infants. (3) Social play, which is a maternal verbal behavior categorized as one of the strategies for getting infants' attention, was significantly related to the acquisition of infants' vocabulary. (4) When mothers used more imitating sounds and mimetic words, requests for information, descriptions, conventional social expressions, and imitation to enhance responsiveness, infants were found to have acquired a larger vocabulary.
Keywords
infant's nonverbal communication; maternal verbal behavior; infant's vocabulary;
Citations & Related Records
Times Cited By KSCI : 2  (Citation Analysis)
연도 인용수 순위
1 Wetherby, A. M., Prizant, B. M., & Hutchinson, T. A. (1998). Communicative, social/affective, and symbolic profiles of young children with autism and pervasive developmental disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7(2), 79-91.   DOI
2 Wikstrom, P. (1994). The Role of Attention in Early Cognitive Development. Unpublished doctoral dissertion, Columbia University, New York, USA.
3 Wilcox, M. J. (1992). Enhancing initial communication skills in young children with developmental disabilities through partner programming. Seminars in Speech and Language, 13(3), 194-212.   DOI
4 Yoder, P. J., Warren, S. F., Kim, K., & Gazdag, G. E. (1994). Facilitating prelinguistic communication skills in young children with developmental delay II : Systematic replication and extension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(4), 841-851.   DOI
5 Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1998). Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1207-1219.   DOI
6 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein,M. H. (1989). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development 60, 738-751.   DOI   ScienceOn
7 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1994). Specificity in Mother-Toddler Language-Play relations across the second year. Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 283-292.   DOI   ScienceOn
8 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748-767.   DOI   ScienceOn
9 Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6), 1454-1463.   DOI   ScienceOn
10 Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4(12), 197-211.   DOI
11 Turner, M., & Masur, E. F. (2001). Stability and consistency in mothers' and infants' interactive styles. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47(1), 100-120.   DOI
12 Vigil, D. C., & Hwa-Froelich, D. A. (2004). Interaction styles in minority caregivers. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 25(3), 119-126.   DOI
13 Vigil, D. C., Hodges, J., & Klee, T. (2005). Quantity and quality of parental language input to late-talking toddlers during play. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21(2), 107-122.   DOI
14 Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C. A., & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from babbling to speech : Common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7(1), 3-40.   DOI
15 Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (1989). The expression of communicative intent : Assessment guidelines. Seminars in Speech and Language, 10(1), 77-89.   DOI
16 Wetherby, A., M., & Prizant, B. M. (2003). Communication and symbolic behavior scales. Baltimore : Paul H Brookes.
17 Power, T. G. (1985). Mother- and father-infant play : A developmental analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1514-1524.   DOI   ScienceOn
18 Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregivers' contingent comments to 9-month-old infants : Relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 221-234.
19 Rome-Flanders, T., & Cronk, C. (1995). A longitudinal study of infant vocalizations during mother-infant games. Journal of Child Language, 22, 259-259.
20 Ross, G., Kagan, J., Zelazo, P., & Kotelchuck, M. (1975). Separation protest in infants in home and laboratory. Developmental Psychology, II, 256-257.
21 Schwartz, R. G. (1988). Phonological factors in early lexical acquisition. In M. D. Smith, & L. L. John (Eds.), The emergent lexicon : The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary, Developmental psychology series (pp. 185- 222). San Diego : Academic Press.
22 Ruddy, M. G., & Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Cognitive correlates of infant attention and maternal stimulation over the first year of life. Child Development, 53, 183-188.   DOI   ScienceOn
23 Ruff, H. A., & Saltarelli, L. M. (1993). Exploratory play with objects : Basic cognitive processes and individual differences. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 59, 5-16.
24 Ryan, J. (1974). Early language development : Towards a communicational analysis. In M. P. M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 185-213). Cambridge : University of Cambridge.
25 Shin, M. K. (2006). Maternal input and responsiveness in the vocabulary development of children at 13 and 20 months. Unpublished master's thesis, Seoul University, Seoul, Korea.
26 Smith, C. B., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1988). Interactional predictors of early language. First Language, 8(23), 143-156.   DOI
27 Spencer, P. E., & Meadow?Orlans, K. P. (1996). Play, language, and maternal responsiveness : A longitudinal study of deaf and hearing infants. Child Development, 67(6), 3176-3191.   DOI   ScienceOn
28 Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers' attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(4), 557-568.   DOI   ScienceOn
29 Nicolich, L. M. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence : Assessment of symbolic maturity through analysis of pretend play. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly : Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 89-99.
30 O'Brien, M., Johnson, J. M., & Anderson-Goetz, D. (1989). Evaluating quality in mother-infant interaction : Situational effects. Infant Behavior and Development, 12(4), 451-464.   DOI   ScienceOn
31 Oller, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J., & Ross, C. (1976). Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 1-11.
32 Owens, R. E. (2005). Language Development : An Introduction. Boston, MA : Allyn & Bacon.
33 Paavola, L., Kunnari, S., & Moilanen, I. (2005). Maternal responsiveness and infant intentional communication : Implications for the early communicative and linguistic development. Child : Care, Health and Development, 31(6), 727-735.   DOI   ScienceOn
34 Paavola, L., Kunnari, S., Moilanen, I., & Lehtihalmes, M. (2005). The functions of maternal verbal responses to prelinguistic infants as predictors of early communicative and linguistic development. First Language, 25(2), 173-195.   DOI   ScienceOn
35 Pae, S. Y. (2003). Validity and Reliability of the Korean Adaptation of MCDI. Korean Journal of Communication Disorders, 8(2), 1-14.
36 Park, S. M., & Jo, H. S. (2008). Maternal conversation strategies in mother-child interactions and infants' early vocabulary development : focused on the contextual effects. The journal of Korea Early Childhood Education, 15(4), 177-204.
37 Pence, K. L., & Justice, L. M. (2010). 언어발달 : 이론에서 실제까지. 김성수.김화수.이상경.황보명(공역). 서울 : 학지사. (2008년 원저 발간).
38 Pine, J. M. (1992). Maternal style at the early one-word stage : Re-evaluating the stereotype of the directive mother. First Language, 12(35), 169-186.   DOI
39 Lee, J. Y., Lee, K. Y., & Chang, Y. K. (2004). The effect of maternal verbal interaction style on infants' early vocabulary development during picture book reading. The Korean journal of development psychology, 17(1), 131-146.
40 Lee, J. Y., Chang, Y. K., Kwak, K. J., Seong, H. R., & Sim, H, O. (2005). The Short-term Longitudinal Study of Maternal Verbal Behaviors in Early Infancy. The Korean journal of development psychology, 18(1), 61-77.
41 Lewis, M., & Freedle, R. (1973). Mother-infant dyad : The cradle of meaning. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Alloway (Eds.), Communication and affect : Language and thought. New York : Academic Press.
42 Lifter, K., & Bloom, L. (1998). Intentionality and the role of play in the transition to language. In A. M. Wetherby, S. F. Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication (pp. 15-37). Baltimore : P. H. Brookes Pub.
43 Locke, J. L., & Pearson, D. M. (1990). Linguistic significance of babbling : Evidence from a tracheostomized infant. Journal of Child Language, 17(1), 1-16.   DOI
44 Matheny, A. P. Jr. (1989). Temperament and cognition : relations between temperament and mental test scores. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates &M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 263-282). West Sussex : John Wiley & Sons.
45 McCune, L., & Vihman, M. M. (2001). Early phonetic and lexical development : A productivity approach. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(3), 670-684.   DOI   ScienceOn
46 Messinger, D. S., & Fogel, A. (1998). Give and take : The development of conventional infant gestures. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 44, 566-590.
47 Nathani, S., Ertmer, D. J., & Stark, R. E. (2006). Assessing vocal development in infants and toddlers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 20(5), 351-369.   DOI   ScienceOn
48 Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and Strategy in Learning to Talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1/2), 1-135.
49 Kita, S., Kantartzis, K., & Imai, M. (2010). Children learn sound symbolic words better : Evolutionary vestige of sound symbolic protolanguage. In A. D. M. Smith, M. Schouwstra, B. Boer, & K. Smith (Eds.), The Evolution of Language (pp. 206-213). Singapore : World Scientific Publishing.
50 Kim, Y. S., & Kwak, K. J. (2010). The Relationship between Maternal Verbal Responsiveness, Infant's Social Communication Ability during Infancy and Language Ability in Early Childhood. The Korean journal of human development, 17(1), 191-207.
51 Kwon, Y. H., & Kim, Y. M. (2011). The characteristic comparison of the communicative gestures and the vocabulary developments in the hearing impaired infants and the normal infants. The Journal of Special Children Education, 13(2), 151-174.
52 Laakso, M. L., Poikkeus, A. M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (1999). Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 541-556.   DOI   ScienceOn
53 Laakso, M. L., Poikkeus, A. M., Katajamaki, J., & Lyytinen, P. (1999). Early intentional communication as a predictor of language development in young toddlers. First Language, 19(56), 207-231.   DOI   ScienceOn
54 Lee, K. O. (2006). Relationship between maternal conversational function and question type and early language development. The Korean journal of community living science, 17(3), 3-14.   과학기술학회마을
55 Lee, Y. J. (2006). The relationship among infants' communication aged 10 to 24 months, mothers' communication, and mother-infant interative responsiveness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea.
56 Lee, Y. J. (2009). The relationship between early communicative skills and vocabulary development, and the effects of early communicative skills on the vocabulary development for toddlers. The Journal of Korea Open Association for Early Childhood Education, 14(1), 151-173.
57 Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young american children. Baltimore, MA : Brookes Publishing.
58 Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it : Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14(1), 23-45.   DOI
59 Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81-103.   DOI   ScienceOn
60 Harding, C. G., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelinguistic infants. Child Development, 50(1), 33-40.   DOI   ScienceOn
61 Hoff, E. (2009). Language development. Stamford, MA : Wadsworth Publishing.
62 Hong, G. H. (2007). A longitudinal study of communicative intention variables for predicting expressive vocabulary development of the “late-talkers”. Journal of early childhood special education, 7(1), 97-115.
63 Imai, M., Kita, S., Nagumo, M., & Okada, H. (2008). Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition, 109(1), 54-65.   DOI   ScienceOn
64 Karrass, J., Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Mullins, J., & Lefever, J. B. (2002). Processes in language acquisition : The roles of gender, attention, and maternal encouragement of attention over time. Journal of Child Language, 29(3), 519-543.
65 Kim, E. H., & Kim, M. S. (2006). The Relationship Between the Communicative Gesture and the Vocabulary Acquisition of Infants. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 27(6), 217-234.
66 Kim, K. J. (2000). The toddler-mother interaction types and toddler language development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duksung Women's University, Seoul, Korea.
67 Crais, E., Douglas, D. D., & Campbell, C. C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 678-694.   DOI   ScienceOn
68 Kim, M. S., & Lee, Y. J. (2007). The Relationship between Mother's Interactive Responsiveness and Infant's Communication. Journal of the Korean Home Economics Association, 45(3), 97-107.
69 Carpenter, R. L., Mastergeorge, A. M., & Coggins, T. E. (1983). The acquisition of communicative intentions in infants eight to fifteen months of age. Language and Speech, 26(2), 101-116.
70 Casby, M. W., & Ruder, K. F. (1983). Symbolic play and early language development in normal and mentally retarded children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26(3), 404-411.   DOI
71 Dixon Jr, W. E., & Smith, P. H. (2000). Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 417-440.
72 Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of Mother-infant social interactions on infants' subsequent contingency task performance. Child Development, 61(3), 785-793.   DOI   ScienceOn
73 Dunham, P.J., & Dunham, F. (1995). Optimal social structures and adaptive infant development. In C. Moore and P.J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention : Its origin and role in development (p. 159-188). Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
74 Fagan, M. K. (2009). Mean length of utterance before words and grammar : Longitudinal trends and developmental implications of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 36(3), 495-527.   DOI   ScienceOn
75 Flynn, V., & Masur, E. (2007). Characteristics of maternal verbal style : Responsiveness and directiveness in two natural contexts. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 519-543.   DOI   ScienceOn
76 Blake, J., Osborne, P., Cabral, M., & Gluck, P. (2003). The development of communicative gestures in japanese infants. First Language, 23(1), 3-20.   DOI   ScienceOn
77 Goldin-Meadow, S., & Morford, M. (1985). Gesture in early child language : Studies of deaf and hearing children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly : Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 145-176.
78 Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2000). How babies talk : The magic and mystery of language in the first three years of life. New York : A Plume Book.
79 Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development : comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6(2), 183-200.
80 Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time : The use of single word utterances before syntax. New York : The Hague Mouton.
81 Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1990). Activities and interactions of mothers and their firstborn infants in the first six months of life : Covariation, stability, continuity, correspondence, and prediction. Child Development, 61, 1206-1217.   DOI
82 Bornstein, M. H., Tamis?LeMonda, C. S., Tal, J., Ludemann, P., Toda, S. Rahn, C. W., Pecheux, M., Azuma, H., & Vardi, D. (1992). Maternal responsiveness to infants in three societies : The united states, france, and japan. Child Development, 63(4), 808-821.   DOI   ScienceOn
83 Bretherton, I. (1984). Representing the social world in symbolic play : Reality and fantasy. In I, Bretherton(Ed.), Symbolic Play : The Development of Social Understanding (pp. 3-41). New York : Academic Press.
84 Brookhart, J., & Hock, E. (1976). The effects of experimental context and experiential background on infants' behavior toward mothers and a stranger. Child Development, 47, 333-340.   DOI   ScienceOn
85 Bruner, J. S. (1975b). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1-19.
86 Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development : A case study. Human Development, 28(1), 40-49.   DOI
87 Bruner, J. S. (1985). Child's talk : Learning to use language. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 1(1), 111-114.   DOI
88 Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E., & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language, 11(33), 345-358.   DOI
89 Capirci, O., Iverson, J., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23(3), 645-673.   DOI
90 Adamson, L. B., & Chance, S. E. (1998). Coordinating attention to people, objects, and language. In A. M. Wetherby, S. F. Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication (pp. 15-37). Baltimore : P. H. Brookes Pub.
91 Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda M, H., & Catherine, S. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20(2), 247-258.   DOI   ScienceOn
92 Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly : Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 205-226.
93 Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004-1019.   DOI