Browse > Article
http://dx.doi.org/10.19066/cogsci.2020.31.4.003

The effect of lyrical and non-lyrical background music on different types of language processing - An ERP study  

Lee, Eun Kyoung (Brain and Humanities Lab, Seoul National University)
Lee, Sung Eun (Brain and Humanities Lab, Seoul National University)
Kwon, Young Sung (Brain and Humanities Lab, Seoul National University)
Publication Information
Korean Journal of Cognitive Science / v.31, no.4, 2020 , pp. 155-178 More about this Journal
Abstract
People read in different settings, including when music is playing in the background. Whether the presence of music facilitates, hinders, or does not affect language processing is an on-going debate in the current literature. The present study used ERPs to examine the influence of music on orthographic, semantic, and syntactic processing by inspecting P2, N400, and P600 responses, respectively. A total of 60 participants judged the correctness of visually presented sentences while listening to music with lyrics, music without lyrics, or in silence. The results showed that the P2 and P600 effects were larger in the silent condition than in the music-with-lyrics condition, while there were no N400 differences among the conditions. This indicates that only lyrical music interferes with orthographic and syntactic language processing, while it has no, or minimal, effect on semantic processing. The results are discussed in relation to the background music interference effect.
Keywords
language processing; background music; Event-Related Potentials (ERPs); N400; P600;
Citations & Related Records
연도 인용수 순위
  • Reference
1 Mitchell, A. H. (1949). The effect of radio programs on silent reading achievement of ninety-one sixth grade students. The Journal of Educational Research, 42(6), 460-470.   DOI
2 Mitra, P., & Coch, D. (2009). A masked priming ERP study of letter processing using single letters and false fonts. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 216-228.   DOI
3 Patel, A. D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P. J. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(6), 717-733.   DOI
4 Perham, N., & Currie, H. (2014). Does listening to preferred music improve reading comprehension performance?. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 279-284.   DOI
5 Anderson, S. A., & Fuller, G. B. (2010). Effect of music on reading comprehension of junior high school students. School Psychology Quarterly, 25(3), 178.   DOI
6 Barber, H., Vergara, M., & Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport, 15(3), 545-548.   DOI
7 Besson, M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., & Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the Brain: Independence of Lyrics and Tunes. Psychological Science, 9(6), 494-498.   DOI
8 Brandt, A. K., Slevc, R., & Gebrian, M. (2012). Music and early language acquisition. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 327.   DOI
9 Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Barber, H. (2005). Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1803-1817.   DOI
10 Cauchard, F., Cane, J. E., & Weger, U. W. (2012). Influence of background speech and music in interrupted reading: An eye tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(3), 381-390.   DOI
11 Chien, P. J., & Chan, S. H. (2015). Old songs can be as fresh as new: an ERP study on lyrics processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 35, 55-67.   DOI
12 Christoffels, I. K., Ganushchak, L., & Koester, D. (2013). Language conflict in translation: An ERP study of translation production. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 646-664.   DOI
13 Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C., & Thierry, G. (2009). The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(50), 21442-21446.   DOI
14 Daltrozzo, J., & Schon, D. (2009). Conceptual processing in music as revealed by N400 effects on words and musical targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1882-1892.   DOI
15 Daltrozzo, J., Tillmann, B., Platel, H., & Schon, D. (2010). Temporal aspects of the feeling of familiarity for music and the emergence of conceptual processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1754-1769.   DOI
16 Dittinger, E., Barbaroux, M., D'Imperio, M., Jäncke, L., Elmer, S., & Besson, M. (2016). Professional music training and novel word learning: From faster semantic encoding to longer-lasting word representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(10), 1584-1602.   DOI
17 Featherstone, C. R., Morrison, C. M., Waterman, M. G., & MacGregor, L. J. (2013). Semantics, syntax or neither? A case for resolution in the interpretation of N500 and P600 responses to harmonic incongruities. PloS one, 8(11), e76600.   DOI
18 Fitzroy, A. B., & Sanders, L. D. (2012). Musical expertise modulates early processing of syntactic violations in language. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 603.   DOI
19 Freeburne, C. M., & Fleischer, M. S. (1952). The effect of music distraction upon reading rate and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 43(2), 101-109.   DOI
20 Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(2), 78-84.   DOI
21 Gordon, R. L., Schon, D., Magne, C., Astésano, C., & Besson, M. (2010). Words and melody are intertwined in perception of sung words: EEG and behavioral evidence. PLoS One, 5(3), e9889.   DOI
22 Hall, J. C. (1952). The effect of background music on the reading comprehension of 278 eighth and ninth grade students. The Journal of Educational Research, 45(6), 451-458.   DOI
23 Herring, D., & Scott, J. (2018). The effect of lyrical and instrumental music on reading comprehension tasks. Journal of Emerging Investigators, 1-6.
24 Hoeks, J. C., Stowe, L. A., & Doedens, G. (2004). Seeing words in context: the interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(1), 59-73.   DOI
25 Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Letnic, A. K. (2012). Fast and loud background music disrupts reading comprehension. Psychology of Music, 40(6), 700-708.   DOI
26 Sammler, D., Koelsch, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Are left fronto-temporal brain areas a prerequisite for normal music-syntactic processing?. Cortex, 47(6), 659-673.   DOI
27 Samson, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (1991). Recognition memory for text and melody of songs after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: Evidence for dual encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 793-804.   DOI
28 Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration of melody and text in memory for songs. Cognition, 16(3), 285-303.   DOI
29 Schon, D., Boyer, M., Moreno, S., Besson, M., Peretz, I., & Kolinsky, R. (2008). Songs as an aid for language acquisition. Cognition, 106(2), 975-983.   DOI
30 Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2007). Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1169-1178.   DOI
31 Vasilev, M. R., Kirkby, J. A., & Angele, B. (2018). Auditory distraction during reading: A Bayesian meta-analysis of a continuing controversy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(5), 567-597.   DOI
32 Ransdell, S. E., & Gilroy, L. (2001). The effects of background music on word processed writing. Computers in Human Behavior, 17(2), 141-148.   DOI
33 Saito, Y., Ishii, K., Sakuma, N., Kawasaki, K., Oda, K., & Mizusawa, H. (2012). Neural substrates for semantic memory of familiar songs: is there an interface between lyrics and melodies?. PloS one, 7(9), e46354.   DOI
34 Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(10), 1631-1643.   DOI
35 Hughes, R. W. (2014). Auditory distraction: A duplex mechanism account. PsyCh Journal, 3(1), 30-41.   DOI
36 Kallinen, K. (2002). Reading news from a pocket computer in a distracting environment: effects of the tempo of background music. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(5), 537-551.   DOI
37 Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.   DOI
38 Kiger, D. M. (1989). Effects of music information load on a reading comprehension task. Perceptual and motor skills, 69(2), 531-534.   DOI
39 Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature neuroscience, 7(3), 302.   DOI
40 Kong, L., Zhang, B., Zhang, J. X., & Kang, C. (2012). P200 can be modulated by orthography alone in reading Chinese words. Neuroscience letters, 529(2), 161-165.   DOI
41 Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study. Nature neuroscience, 4(5), 540-545.   DOI
42 Marsh, J. E., & Jones, D. M. (2010). Cross-modal distraction by background speech: What role for meaning?. Noise and Health, 12(49), 210.   DOI
43 Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2008). Auditory distraction in semantic memory: A process-based approach. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 682-700.   DOI
44 Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2009). Interference by process, not content, determines semantic auditory distraction. Cognition, 110(1), 23-38.   DOI
45 Martin, R. C., Wogalter, M. S., & Forlano, J. G. (1988). Reading comprehension in the presence of unattended speech and music. Journal of memory and language, 27(4), 382-398.   DOI
46 Miller, L. R. (1947). Some effects of radio-listening on the efficiency of reading-type study activities. Journal of educational psychology, 38(2), 105.   DOI