Browse > Article
http://dx.doi.org/10.7466/JKHMA.2016.34.5.83

The Relationship among Toddler's Symbolic Play Level, Mother's Responsiveness and Verbal Interaction  

Choi, Yeonsoo (Department of Child & Family Stuidies, Yonsei University)
Kim, Myoungsoon (Department of Child & Family Stuidies, Yonsei University)
Pae, Sunyoung (Department of Child Care & Education, Baewha Women's University)
Publication Information
Journal of Families and Better Life / v.34, no.5, 2016 , pp. 83-98 More about this Journal
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship among toddler's symbolic play level, mother's responsiveness and verbal interaction. The subjects were 56 pairs of 24-29 months old toddlers and their mothers. The toddler's symbolic play level was classified using categories by Gowen(1995). The mother's verbal interaction was coded during a 10-minute-free play session with her toddler using Caldera, Huston, and O'Brien's(1989) categories. PICCOLO(The Parenting Interactions with Child Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes) by Roggman, Cook, Innocenti, Jump, Norman, & Christiansen(2009) was used to investigate the mother's responsiveness. While the mothers played with their toddlers during a 10-minute-free play session, their interaction were videotaped. The data were analyzed by means, t-test, and correlations. The results of the study were as follows: First, the mean levels of the toddler's symbolic play were in level 3, 'the pretending others'. Also girls spent more symbolic play time with mothers than boys. Second, mothers' responsiveness was generally high in free play with their toddlers. Mothers frequently used 'questions' 'comments' and 'fantasy statements'. Mothers with girls used more 'questions' than mothers with boys. Third, there was a significant relationship among toddler's symbolic play level, mother's responsiveness and verbal interaction. Maternal responsiveness and 'fantasy statements' were positively related with the mean level, and the frequency of play. The 'questions' and 'answers' were also related with symbolic play level and frequency. But 'praise' was negatively related with symbolic play frequency. In conclusion, this study suggested that toddler's symbolic play level was related with mother's responsiveness and verbal interaction during free play.
Keywords
toddler's symbolic play level; maternal responsiveness; mother's verbal interactions;
Citations & Related Records
Times Cited By KSCI : 3  (Citation Analysis)
연도 인용수 순위
1 McDonald, L., & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behavior as a function of interactional intent. Journal of Child Language, 9(2), 337-358.
2 McLoyd, V. C. (1980). Verbally expressed modes of transformation in the fantasy play of black preschool children. Child Development, 51(6), 1138-1139.
3 Min, M., & Lee, S. (2004). Level of pretend play and comprehension of pretense among 2-and 4-year-old children. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 25(6), 337-354.
4 Moon, H. (1996). A Study on the developmental changes in pretend play from 20 to 35months of age. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Sookmyung Women's University. Seoul, Korea.
5 Morrissey, A. M., & Brown, P. M. (2009). Mother and toddler activity in the zone of proximal developmental for pretend play as a prediction of higher child IQ. Gifted Child Quarterly, 52(2), 106-120.
6 Newland, L. A., Roggman, L. A., & Boyce, L. K. (2001). The development of social toy play and language in infancy. Infant Behavior & Development, 24(1), 1-25.   DOI
7 Nicolich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorymotor intelligence: Analysis of symbolic maturity through analysis of pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 23(1), 89-99.
8 Nielson, M. & Christie, T. (2008). Adult modeling facilitates young children’s generation of novel pretend acts. Infant and Child Development, 17(1), 151-162.   DOI
9 Gowen, J. W. (1995). The early development of symbolic play. Young Children, 50, 75-81.
10 Haight, W. L., & Miller, P. J. (1993). Pretending at home: Early development in a sociocultural context. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
11 Harrist, A. W., & Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Development Review, 22(2), 555-592.   DOI
12 Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782-796.   DOI
13 Honig, A. S. (2006). What infants, toddlers, and preschoolers learn from play: 12 ideas. Montessori Life, 1, 16-21.
14 Howes, C., & Smith, E. W. (1995). Relations among child care quality, teacher behavior, children’s play activities, emotional security, and cognitive activity in child care. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10(4), 381-404.   DOI
15 Kim, Y.-S. (2002). Representational aspect of early pretense: The relation between the symbolic level of present play and theory of mind. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Dongduk Women's University. Seoul, Korea.
16 Howes, C., Unger, O., & Seigner, L. B. (1989). Social pretend play in toddlers: Parallels with social play and with solitary pretend. Child Development, 60(1), 77-84.   DOI
17 Johnson, J. E., Christie, J. F., & Yawkey, T. D. (1999). Play and early childhood development. Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Froesman and Company.
18 Kim, G. (2000). The toddler-mother interaction types and toddler language development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Duksung Women's University. Seoul, Korea.
19 Kim, M., & Chae, Y. (1999). Mother-infant book reading in the home. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 20(2), 125-138.
20 Noh, J. (2012). The relationship between mother-infant bookreading behaviors and infant's vocabulary aged 12 to 18 months. Unpublished master's thesis. Yonsei University. Seoul, Korea.
21 Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Radziszewska, B., & Germond, J. (1992). Infants' instrumental social interaction with adults. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 57-77). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
22 Noll, L. M., & Harding, C. G. (2003). The relationship of mother-child interaction and the child’s development of symbolic play. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24(6), 557-570.   DOI
23 Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1984). Mother-infant interaction and the development of individual differences in children’s cognitive competence. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 166-179.   DOI
24 Roggman, L. A., Cook, G., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., & Christiansen, K. (2009). PICCOLO(Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observacions Linked to Outcomes). Logan, UT: Utah State University.
25 Rossetti, L. M. (2001). Communication intervention: Birth to three. Oshkosh, Wisconsin: Singular.
26 Shin, E., Kim, M., Shin, D., Lee, J., & Choi, S. (2002). Play and young children. Seoul: Ewha women's university publisher.
27 Singer, J. L. (1973). The child's world of make-believe. New York, NY: Academic Press.
28 Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43(3), 549-565.   DOI
29 Stern, D. N. (2002). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
30 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. (1991). Individual variation, correspondence, stability, and change in mother and toddler play. Infant and Development, 14(1), 143-162.   DOI
31 Brazelton, T. B., & Cramer, B. G. (1990). The Earliest Relationship: Parents, infant, and the drama of early attachment. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.
32 Alton, C. (2011). Facilitating infants’sensory development. Exchange, 197(1), 50-54.
33 Belsky, J., & Most, R. (1981). From exploration to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 630-639.   DOI
34 Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimentional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 867-874.   DOI
35 Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs. Washington D. C.: NAEYC.
36 Bretherton, I. (1992). Social referencing, intentional communication, and the interfacing of minds in infancy. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 57-77). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
37 Bruner, J. S. (1983). Play, thought, and language. Peabody Journal of Education, 60(3), 60-69.   DOI
38 Kim, M., & Sung J. (2002). The relationship between infant's language and play. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 23(5), 19-34.
39 Kim, M., & Gwak, K. (2004). Responsiveness and initiative through infant-mother interaction in the social toy play. The Korean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(2), 19-35. 
40 Kim, M., Hyun, O., & Rah, J. (2007). A short-term longitudinal study on infant and mother interaction, Journal of Korean Home Management Association 25(5), 167-183.
41 Ko, J., & Lee, S. (2010). A comparison of mothers' and fathers' responsiveness while playing with their two-year-old children. Korean Journal of Early Childhood Education, 30(2), 147-165.
42 Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68(1), 94-112.   DOI
43 Lim, Y. (2002). Korean infants' vocabulary development and mothers' verbal behaviors. Unpublished master's thesis. Yonsei University. Seoul, Korea.
44 Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddler with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60(3), 70-76.   DOI
45 Laakso, M. L., Poikkeus, A. M., & Lyytinen, P. (1999). Shared reading interaction in families with and without genetic risk for dyslexia: Implications for toddler’s language development. Infant and Child Development, 8(2), 179-195.   DOI
46 Landry, S. H., Smith, K., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vallet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children's development or in consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 387-403.   DOI
47 Lee, M. (1999). Mother-infant interactions in social games. Unpublished master's thesis. Yonsei University. Seoul, Korea.
48 Lee, Y., & Kim, M. (2013). The relationships among infants’nonverbal communication, maternal verbal behaviors and infants’acquisition of vocabulary. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 34(2), 1-25.   DOI
49 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Maternal responsiveness and early language acquisition. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 29(1), 89-127.   DOI
50 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. (1995). Variation in children's exploration, nonsymbolic, and symbolic play: An explanatory. In C. Collier & L. Lipsett (Eds.), Advances in infancy research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
51 Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(5), 1454-1463.   DOI
52 Trawick-Smith, J. (2007). Interactions in the Classroom. Columbus, OH: Merill Prentice Hall.
53 van Hoorn, J., Nourot, P. M., Scales, B., & Alward, K. R. (2003). Play at the center of the curriculum. Columbus, OH: Merrill Prentice Hall.
54 Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J. S. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 537-554). New York, NY: Basic.
55 Walker-Andrews, A., & Kahana-Kalman, R. (1999). The understanding of pretence across the second year of life. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 523-536.   DOI
56 Woo, S. H. (2004). Research about make-believe factors in symbolic play of the classes with one and two-year-old children. Unpublished master's thesis. Duksung Women's University. Seoul, Korea.
57 Yoon, E. (2010). The relationship among parents play belief, mother's verbal interaction and toddler's play level. Unpublished master's thesis. Yonsei University. Seoul, Korea.
58 Cho, S. (2010). The relation of toddler's playfulness, play level and maternal verbal interaction. Unpublished master's thesis. Yonsei University. Seoul, Korea.
59 Calkins, S. D., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Toddler regulation of distress to frustrating events: Temperamental and maternal correlates. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(5), 379-395.
60 Carew, J. V. (1980). Experience and the development of intelligence in young children in home and in day care. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 45(1), 1-89.   DOI
61 Cohen, S. E., & Beckwith, L. (1979). Preterm-infant interaction with the caregiver in the first year of life and competence at age two. Child Development, 50(6), 767-776.   DOI
62 Conner, D. B., & Cross, D. R. (2003). Longitudinal analysis of the presence, efficacy and stability of maternal scaffolding during informal problem-solving interactions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 315-334.   DOI
63 Cross, T. G. (1984). Mother’s speech adjustments: the contribution of play maturity in preschool children. Early Child Development 14(1), 147-160.   DOI
64 Damast, A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. (1996). Mother-child play: Sequential interactions and the relation between maternal beliefs and behaviors. Child Development, 67(6), 1752-1766.   DOI
65 Erickson, S. J., Montague, E. Q., Maclean, P. C., Bancroft, M. E., & Lowe, J. R. (2012). Differential ethnic associations between maternal flexibility and play sophistication in toddlers born very low birth weight. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 860-869.   DOI
66 De Wolff, M. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68(4), 571-591.   DOI
67 Dixon Jr, W. E., & Smith, H. P. (2003). Who’s controlling whom? Infant contributions to maternal play behavior. Infant and Child Development, 12(2), 177-195.   DOI
68 Dixon, W., & Shore, C. (1993). What shall we play? Play content as a vehicle for parent-infant interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(11), 121-125.   DOI
69 Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1992). Lexical development during middle infancy: A mutually driven infant-caregiver process. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 414-420.   DOI
70 Dunn, J., & Dale, N. (1984). I and daddy: 2-year-old's collaboration in joint pretend with sibling and with mother. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic play: The development of social understanding (pp.131-158). New York, NY: Academic.
71 Faver, J. M., & Wimbarti, S. (1995). Indonesian toddler’s social play with their mothers and older siblings. Child Development, 66(5), 1493-1503.   DOI
72 Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Development, 52(4), 1095-1118.   DOI
73 Fein, G. G., Johnson, D., Kosson, N., Stork L., & Wasserman, L. (1975). Stereotypes and preferences in the toy choices of 20-month-old boys and girls. Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 527-528.   DOI
74 Frodi, U., Bridges, L., & Grolnick, W. (1985). Correlates of mastery behavior: A short-term longitudinal study. Child Development, 40(1), 21-31.   DOI
75 Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A. M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). The play and language behavior of mothers with and without dyslexia and its association to their toddlers’ language development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 74-86.   DOI
76 McGee, L. M., & Richgels, D. J. (1996). Literacy's beginnings: Supporting young readers and writers. Seoul: Hakjisa.
77 Manning, K., & Sharp, A. (1997). Structuring play in the early years at school. London, London: Ward Lock Educational.
78 Fenson, L. (1984). Developmental trends for action and speech in pretend play. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic play: The development of social understanding (pp.249-269). New York:, NY: Academic.
79 Fiese, B. H. (1990). Playful relationship: A contextual analysis of mother-toddler interaction and symbolic play, Child Development, 61(6), 1648-1656.   DOI
80 Go, J. (2003). Mother's linguistic types in activity of reading picture-books and babies' reactions. Unpublished master's thesis. Suwon University. Gyeonggi, Korea.