DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Self-distancing as a Cognitive Tool for Emotion Regulation: Its Concept, Measurement, and Effect

인지적 정서조절 기제로서 자기거리두기: 개념, 측정과 효과 소개

  • Received : 2020.09.04
  • Accepted : 2020.09.24
  • Published : 2020.09.30

Abstract

This study reviewed the literature on the self-distancing method for emotion regulation. Self-distancing refers to taking a step back in dealing with one's own experiences and interpreting them in a relatively objective manner. Studies have indicated that self-distancing affects cognitive and emotional processes, which are particularly beneficial in regulating negative emotions. Therefore, this study focused on organizing previous results to summarize their meaning, assess means of measurement, and analyze possible effects. Above all, the related concepts of an observer's rational perspective and self-reflection were presented to clarify the definition of self-distancing, followed by a discussion of its measurement. Based on this, the effects of self-distancing were introduced according to their emotional, cognitive, physiological, developmental, and behavioral aspects. However, despite repeated verification of the positive effects of self-distancing, there are several inconsistencies that require consideration. Finally, we suggested the study's academic and practical implications and recommended directions for further research.

본 논문에서는 인지적 정서조절의 방법으로 활용되는 자기거리두기의 개념과 측정, 효과에 대해 소개하였다. 자기거리두기(self-distancing)는 자신에게 몰입하는 상태의 반대 개념으로, 자신의 경험을 다룰 때 한 발짝 물러나서 이를 다소 관조적으로 바라보는 행위를 일컫는다. 자신의 경험을 다룰 때 마음속에서 자신과 얼마나 거리를 두는지에 따라 인지적 정서적으로 지각되는 정도가 다르고 이는 사건을 이해하는 정도는 물론 이로 인한 정서의 양상에까지 영향을 준다. 그 간의 많은 연구들에 따르면, 자기거리두기를 많이 하는 것이 개인의 정서에 도움이 된다는 결과들이 지배적인데, 본 논문에서는 이러한 결과들을 정리하여 자기거리두기가 무엇을 의미하고 어떤 효과가 있는지를 정리해 보았다. 먼저 본 연구에서는 이러한 자기거리두기 개념의 이해를 돕기 위해 관찰자 관점, 자기반성, 이성적 태도 등의 개념을 통해 자기거리두기가 어떤 특징이 있고 어떤 방향에서 바라보고 접근하게 되는지를 제시하였고, 이와 함께 자기거리두기를 어떻게 측정할 수 있는지 또한 짚어보았다. 그리고 이를 바탕으로 자기거리두기로부터 얻을 수 있는 효과를 정서적, 인지적, 생리적, 행동적, 발달적 측면에서 세분화하여 소개하였다. 한편, 이 개념으로부터 기대되는 긍정적인 효과들이 반복적으로 검증됨에도 불구하고 일관되지 않은 결과들도 함께 발견되고 있어 이를 함께 언급하였다. 아울러 자기거리두기가 앞으로 연구되는 과정에서 다루어져야 하는 부분들에 대한 고찰이 함께 이루어졌다. 마지막으로 이연구가 가진 학술적·실용적 의미들을 짚어보았다.

Keywords

References

  1. An, D., & Lee, H-J. (2016). Psychological construct of zhongyong (Doctrine of the mean): A preliminary study. Korean Journal of Psychology: General, 35(2), 309-326. https://doi.org/10.22257/kjp.2016.06.35.2.309
  2. An, D., & Lee, H-J. (2017). Development and validation of the Zhongyong Questionnaire. Korean Journal of Psychology: General, 36(4), 421-440. https://doi.org/10.22257/kjp.2017.09.36.4.421
  3. Anderson, C. A., Buckley, K. E., & Carnagey, N. L. (2008). Creating your own hostile environment: A laboratory examination of trait aggression and the violence escalation cycle. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(4), 462-473. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207311282
  4. Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10(3), 229-240. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229
  5. Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of recovery: Differential effects of analyzing negative experiences from a self-distanced vs. self-immersed perspective on blood pressure reactivity. Psychological Science, 19(3), 229-231. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02073.x
  6. Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2010). From a distance: implications of spontaneous self-distancing for adaptive self-reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(5), 809-829. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019205
  7. Ayduk, O., Mischel, W., & Downey, G. (2002). Attentional mechanisms linking rejection to hostile reactivity: The role of “hot” versus “cool” focus. Psychological Science, 13(5), 443-448. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00478
  8. Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2008). The fascination of wisdom: Its nature, ontogeny, and function. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 56-64. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00062.x
  9. Basseches, M. (1980). Dialectical schemata: A framework for the empirical study of the development of dialectical thinking. Human Development, 23, 400-421. https://doi.org/10.1159/000272600
  10. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2006). Emotion and vantage point in autobiographical memory. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1193-1215. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500371190
  11. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the selfregulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  12. Cho, M. H. (2018). The effects of third-person perspective on emotion regulation through self-distancing. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Sogang University, Seoul, Korea.
  13. Cho, M. H., & Na, J. (2017). The effect of mood awareness and emotional expressivity on the relationship between academic stress and adjustment. Science of Emotion and Sensibility, 20(2), 45-58. https://doi.org/10.14695/KJSOS.2017.20.2.45
  14. Choi, S.-E., & Choi, H-S. (2014). Effects of Visual Perspectives and Construal Level on Social Anxiety. The Korean Journal of Health Psychology, 19(1), 208-301.
  15. Choi, Y. I., & Kwon, J. H. (2009). The effect of observer perspective on speaking anxiety and speaking performance. Korean Journal of Psychology: General, 28(1), 147-166.
  16. Cohen, D., & Gunz, A. (2002). As seen by the other…: Perspectives on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Westerners. Psychological Science, 13(1), 55-59. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00409
  17. Coles, M. E., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., & Fresco, D. M. (2001). Effects of varying levels of anxiety within social situations: Relationship to memory perspective and attributions in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(6), 651-665. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00035-8
  18. Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hope, D. A., & Schneier, F. R. (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. (pp. 69-93). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  19. Craft, A., Gardner, H., & Claxton, G. (Eds.). (2008). Creativity, wisdom, and trusteeship: Exploring the role of education. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
  20. Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between experiential and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819-829. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819
  21. Dorfman, A., Oakes, H., Santos, H. C., & Grossmann, I. (2019). Self-distancing promotes emotional growth after adversity: Evidence from a micro-longitudinal field experiment.
  22. Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 165-192). New York: Guilford Press.
  23. Freud, S. (1953). Screen memories. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.) Sigmund Freud: Collected papers (Vol. 6, pp. 43-63). London: Hogarth Press.
  24. Fuentes, A. M. M., Kahn, J. H., & Lannin, D. G. (2018). Emotional disclosure and emotion change during an expressive-writing task: Do pronouns matter?. Current Psychology, 1-8.
  25. Giovanetti, A. K., Revord, J. C., Sasso, M. P., & Haeffel, G. J. (2019). Self-distancing may be harmful: Third-person writing increases levels of depressive symptoms compared to traditional expressive writing and no writing. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 50-69. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2019.38.1.50
  26. Go, Y. R. (2016). The effect of self-distancing on anger control. (Unpublished master's thesis). Ajou University, Suwon, Korea.
  27. Gross, J. J. (2015). The extended process model of emotion regulation: Elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 130-137. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751
  28. Grossmann, I. (2017). Wisdom in context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 233-257. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616672066
  29. Grossmann, I., & Kross, E. (2014). Exploring Solomon’s Paradox: Self-distancing eliminates the self-other asymmetry in wise reasoning about close relationships in younger and older adults. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1571-1580. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614535400
  30. Grossmann, I., Na, J., Varnum, M. E., Park, D. C., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(16), 7246-7250. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001715107
  31. Grossmann, I., Sahdra, B. K., & Ciarrochi, J. (2016). A heart and a mind: Self-distancing facilitates the association between heart rate variability, and wise reasoning. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(68), 1-10.
  32. Gruber, J. Harvey, A. G., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Reflective and ruminative processing of positive emotional memories in bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(8), 697-704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.005
  33. Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48(3), 263-274. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00073-4
  34. Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006
  35. Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1985). Selfconcept discrepancy theory: A psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression and anxiety. Social cognition, 3(1), 51-76. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1985.3.1.51
  36. James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion. Psychological Review, 1, 516-529. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0065078
  37. Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression: tests and an extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(2), 221-229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.221
  38. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delta.
  39. Kihlstrom, J. F., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1982). The earliest recollection: A new survey. Journal of Personality, 50(2), 134-148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1982.tb01019.x
  40. Kim, H. R. (2013). Effects of sensitivity in identification of other's emotion and self-distancing on change of negative affects, (Unpublished master's thesis). The Catholic University of Korea, Bucheon, Korea.
  41. Kramer, D. (2000). Wisdom as a classical source of human strength: Conceptualization and empirical inquiry. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1), 83-101. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2000.19.1.83
  42. Kross, E. (2009). When the self becomes other. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167(1), 35-40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04545.x
  43. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2008). Facilitating adaptive emotional analysis: Distinguishing distanced-analysis of depressive experiences from immersed-analysis and distraction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(7), 924-938. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208315938
  44. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2009). Boundary conditions and buffering effects: Does depressive symptomology moderate the effectiveness of self-distancing for facilitating adaptive emotional analysis?. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5), 923-927. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.004
  45. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 187-191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411408883
  46. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2017). Self-distancing: Theory, research, and current directions. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 55, pp. 81-136). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.10.002
  47. Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking ‘‘why’’ does not hurt: Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16(9), 709-715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01600.x
  48. Kross, E., Duckworth, A., Ayduk, O., Tsukayama, E., & Mischel, W. (2011). The effect of self-distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection in children. Emotion, 11(5), 1032-1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021787
  49. Kross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, O. (2012). "Asking why" from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 559. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028808
  50. Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Response to depression in unipolar depressed patients: An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles theory. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 1323-1333. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291799001282
  51. Kuyken, W., & Moulds, M. L. (2009). Remembering as an observer: How is autobiographical memory retrieval vantage perspective linked to depression?. Memory, 17(6), 624-634. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902984526
  52. Lange, C. G. (1885). The mechanism of the emotions. The Classical Psychologists, 672-684.
  53. Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: Conceptual and empirical relations. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 16 (pp. 175-270). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  54. Leitner, J. B., Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Magerman, A., Amey, R., Kross, E., & Forbes, C. E. (2017). Self-distancing improves interpersonal perceptions and behavior by decreasing medial prefrontal cortex activity during the provision of criticism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 534-543. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw168
  55. Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2002). Looking back in time: Self-concept change affects visual perspective in autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 82(2), 167-179. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.2.167
  56. Lim, N. (2008). The effect of self-perception perspectives on self-justification motivation and lay rationalis, (Unpublished master's thesis). Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea.
  57. McIsaac, H. K., & Eich, E. (2004). Vantage point in traumatic memory. Psychological Science, 15(4), 248-253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00660.x
  58. McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion, 20(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000703
  59. Metcalfe, J., & Jacobs, W.J. (1998). Emotional memory: The effects of stress on "cool" and "hot" memory systems. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 38. Advances in research and theory (pp. 187-222). SanDiego, CA: Academic Press.
  60. Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3-19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3
  61. Mischkowski, D., Kross, E., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Flies on the wall are less aggressive: Self-distancing "in the heat of the moment" reduces aggressive thoughts, angry feelings and aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(5), 1187-1191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.012
  62. Moser, J. S., Dougherty, A., Mattson, W. I., Katz, B., Moran, T. P., Guevarra, D., Shablack, H., Ayduk, O., Jonides, J., Berman, M. G, & Kross, E. (2017). Third-person self-talk facilitates emotion regulation without engaging cognitive control: Converging evidence from ERP and fMRI. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x
  63. Moser, J. S., Krompinger, J. W., Dietz, J., & Simons, R. F. (2009). Electrophysiological correlates of decreasing and increasing emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 17-27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00721.x
  64. Neff, K. (2003). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2(2), 85-101. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860309032
  65. Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15(4), 467-482. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90016-6
  66. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569-582. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569
  67. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504-511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504
  68. Park, J., Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2016). Stepping back to move forward: Expressive writing promotes selfdistancing. Emotion, 16(3), 349-364. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000121
  69. Park, J., Ayduk, O., O’Donnell, L., Chun, J., Gruber, J., Kamali, M., McInnis, M. G., Deldin, P., & Kross, E. (2014). Regulating the high: Cognitive and neural processes underlying positive emotion regulation in bipolar I disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(6), 661-674. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614527580
  70. Park, J. H. (2006). External self and cognition and emotion, (Unpublished master' thesis). Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea.
  71. Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion. A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.
  72. Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 427-441. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003096
  73. Robinson, J. A., & Swanson, K. K. (1993). Field and observer modes of remembering. Memory, 1(3), 169-184. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658219308258230
  74. Ruiz-Padial , E., Sollers, J. J., Vila, J., & Thayer, J. F. (2003). The rhythm of the heart in the blink of an eye: Emotion‐modulated startle magnitude covaries with heart rate variability. Psychophysiology, 40(2), 306-313. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00032
  75. Safran, J. D., & Segal, Z. V. (1990). Cognitive therapy: An interpersonal process perspective. New York: Basic.
  76. Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 49-80). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60048-9
  77. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379-399. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046234
  78. Segerstrom, S. C., Hardy, J. K., Evans, D. R., and Winters, N. F. (2011). "'Pause and plan': self-regulation and the heart," in Motivational Perspectives on Cardiovascular Response, eds G. Gendolla and R. Wright (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 181-198.
  79. Sloper, P. (2000). Predictors of distress in parents of children with cancer: A prospective study. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 25, 79-91. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/25.2.79
  80. Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2003). The observer perspective: effects on social anxiety and performance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(9), 1009-1028. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00177-8
  81. Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275-287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.275
  82. Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., & Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: The neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 141-153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z
  83. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private selfconsciousness and the Five-Factor Model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284-304. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284
  84. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247-259. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561
  85. Verduyn, P., Van Mechelen, I., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2011). The relation between event processing and the duration of emotional experience. Emotion, 11, 20-28. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021239
  86. Waugh, C. E., Hamilton, J. P., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). The neural temporal dynamics of the intensity of emotional experience. Neuro Image, 49, 1699-1707.
  87. Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 117-142. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.117
  88. Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Effect of visual perspective on memory and interpretation in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49 (6-7), 406-412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.03.012