DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

노화에 의해 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력이 정서지각 편향성에 미치는 영향

The Influences of Deteriorated Visuo-spatial Attention Allocation Ability Caused by Aging on Emotional Perception Bias

  • 투고 : 2020.07.07
  • 심사 : 2020.10.08
  • 발행 : 2020.12.31

초록

본 연구의 목적은 노화에 따라 나타나는 시공간 주의배분능력과 정서지각 편향성의 변화에 대해 탐구하는 것이다. 본 연구에서는 시공간 주의배분능력을 측정하기 위해 useful field of view (UFOV) 과제와 정서지각 편향성을 측정하기 위해 정서지각과제(emotional perception task)를 사용하였다. 본 연구에는 노인 집단의 23명과 청년 집단의 25명의 총 48명의 실험참가자가 참여하였다. 실험 결과, UFOV 과제에서는 노인 집단이 청년 집단에 비해 느린 반응속도와 낮은 정답률을 보여 노인 집단이 청년 집단보다 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력을 가지고 있다는 것을 나타냈다. 또한, 정서지각과제에서는 노인 집단이 청년 집단에 비해 긍정적 정서지각 편향성과 부정적 정서지각 편향성 모두 크게 나타났다. 상관분석 결과에서는 정서지각과제의 정답률에 대한 부정적 정서지각 편향성이 UFOV 과제의 시야각 30° 조건의 반응속도와 유의미한 정적 상관관계가 나타났다(r=.289). 또한, 정서지각과제의 정답률에 대한 긍정적 정서지각 편향성은 UFOV 과제의 모든 시야각 조건들의 정답률과 유의미한 정적 상관관계가 나타났으며(r=.305, r=.322, r=.299), 반응속도와 유의미한 부적 상관관계가 나타났다(r=-.345, r=-.295, r=-.308). 이러한 결과는 노화가 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력 그리고 긍정적 정서지각 편향성과 부정적 정서지각 편향성의 증가와 관련있다는 것을 의미하고, 노화에 따라 나타나는 긍정적 및 부정적 정서지각 편향성은 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력과 관련성이 있는 것으로 여겨진다.

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of aging on visuo-spatial attention allocation ability and emotional perception bias. We used the useful field of view (UFOV) task to measure the visuo-spatial attention allocation ability and the emotional perception task to measure positive and negative emotional perception bias. A total of 48 participants took part in this study with 23 participants in the senior group and 25 in the junior group. The senior group showed slower response time and lower accuracy than the junior group in the UFOV task, indicating that the senior group had lower visuo-spatial attention allocation ability than the junior group. In the emotional perception task, the senior group showed both positive and negative emotional perception bias more than the junior group. The correlation analysis showed that the negative emotional perception bias for accuracy in the emotional perception task showed a positive correlation with the response time to the stimuli presented in the visual angle 30° in the UFOV task (r=.289). In addition, positive emotional perception bias for the accuracy in the emotional perception task showed a positive correlation with the accuracy of the stimuli presented in the visual angles 10°, 20°, and 30° in the UFOV task (r=.305, r=.322, and r=.299, respectively). However, it showed a negative correlation with the response time of the stimuli presented in the same location in the UFOV task (r=-.345, r=-.295, r=-.308). These results suggest that aging is associated with a decrease in the visuo-spatial attention allocation ability and perceptual bias toward positive and negative emotions. In addition, the positive and negative emotional perception biases associated with aging are potentially related to the reduced visuo-spatial attention allocation ability.

키워드

과제정보

본 연구는 과학기술정보통신부 및 정보통신기획평가원의 대학ICT연구센터육성지원사업의 연구결과로 수행되었음(ITP-2020-2017-0-01630).

참고문헌

  1. Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 817-826. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00178-6
  2. Awh, E., & Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(3), 119-126. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01593-x
  3. Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. L., & Griggs, D. S. (1988). Age and visual search: Expanding the useful field of view. Journal of Optical Society of America A, 5(12), 2210-2219. DOI: 10.1364/josaa.5.002210
  4. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370. DOI: 10.4135/9781412956253.n55
  5. Belham, F. S., Satler, C., Garcia, A., Tomaz, C., Gasbarri, A., Rego, A., & Tavares, M. C. H. (2013). Age-related differences in cortical activity during a visuo-spatial working memory task with facial stimuli. PLoS One, 8(9), e75778. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075778
  6. Bieliauskas, L. A. (2001). General cognitive changes with aging. In Behavioral neurology in the elderly (pp. 85-108). CRC Press, Boca, Boca Raton. DOI: 10.1201/b14249-5
  7. Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Keane, J., & Dean, M. (2000). Configural information in facial expression perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 527. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.527
  8. Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manly, T., Sprengelmeyer, R., Scott, S., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Young, A. W. (2003). Facial expression recognition across the adult life span. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 195-202. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00149-5
  9. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socio-emotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.54.3.165
  10. Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 117-121. DOI: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x
  11. Clapp, W. C., Rubens, M. T., Sabharwal, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Deficit in switching between functional brain networks underlies the impact of multitasking on working memory in older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17), 7212-7217. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1015297108
  12. Coeckelbergh, T. R., Cornelissen, F. W., Brouwer, W. H., & Kooijman, A. C. (2004). Age-related changes in the functional visual field: Further evidence for an inverse age×eccentricity effect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(1), 11-18. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/59.1.p11
  13. Cohn, N. B., Dustman, R. E., & Bradford, D. C. (1984). Age-related decrements in stroop color test performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(5), 1244-1250. DOI: 10.1002/1097-4679(198409)40:5 <1244::aid-jclp2270400521>3.0.co;2-d
  14. Comalli Jr, P. E., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100(1), 47-53. DOI: 10.1080/00221325.1962.10533572
  15. Czigler, I., Csibra, G., & Ambro, A. (1996). Aging, stimulus identification and the effect of probability: an event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 43(1), 27-40. DOI: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05173-2
  16. Donkers, F. C., & Van Boxtel, G. J. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 165-176. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005
  17. Duzel, E., Penny, W. D., & Burgess, N. (2010). Brain oscillations and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 143-149. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.004
  18. Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550-553. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.550
  19. Estes, Z., & Adelman, J. S. (2008). Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general. Emotion, 8, 453-457. DOI: 10.1037/a0012887
  20. Falkenstein, M., Koshlykova, N. A., Kiroj, V. N., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (1995). Late ERP components in visual and auditory Go/Nogo tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 96(1), 36-43. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)00182-k
  21. Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (2002). Inhibition-related ERP components: variation with modality, age, and time-on-task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 16(3), 167. DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803.16.3.167
  22. Fallgatter, A. J., Mueller, T. J., & Strik, W. K. (1999). Age-related changes in the brain electrical correlates of response control. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(5), 833-838. DOI: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00022-x
  23. Gajewski, P. D., Stoerig, P., & Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP-correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Research, 1189, 127-134. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.076
  24. Garcia-Larrea, L., Lukaszewicz, A. C., & Mauguiere, F. (1992). Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia, 30(8), 723-741. DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90042-k
  25. Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129-135. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014
  26. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Foa, E. B., & Amir, N. (1999). Attentional biases for facial expressions in social phobia: The face-in-the-crowd paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 13(3), 305-318. DOI: 10.1080/026999399379294
  27. Gola, M., Kaminski, J., Brzezicka, A., & Wrobel, A. (2012). Beta band oscillations as a correlate of alertness-changes in aging. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(1), 62-67. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.001
  28. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1465. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1465
  29. Greenwood, P. M., Parasuraman, R., & Alexander, G. E. (1997). Controlling the focus of spatial attention during visual search: Effects of advanced aging and Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 3. DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.3
  30. Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (1999). Scale of attentional focus in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(5), 837-859. DOI: 10.3758/bf03206901
  31. Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2004). The scaling of spatial attention in visual search and its modification in healthy aging. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(1), 3-22. DOI: 10.3758/bf03194857
  32. Gruhn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: Evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 579. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579
  33. Hartley, A. A., Kieley, J. M., & Slabach, E. H. (1990). Age differences and similarities in the effects of cues and prompts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 523. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.523
  34. Hammerer, D., Li, S. C., Muller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). An electrophysiological study of response conflict processing across the lifespan: Assessing the roles of conflict monitoring, cue utilization, response anticipation, and response suppression. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), 3305-3316. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.014
  35. Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 781-787. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.781
  36. Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., McDonaldMiszczak, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. Psychology and Aging, 7(4), 571. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.4.571
  37. Hulur, G., Ram, N., Willis, S. L., Schaie, K. W., & Gerstorf, D. (2015). Cognitive dedifferentiation with increasing age and proximity of death: Within-person evidence from the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Psychology and Aging, 30(2), 311. DOI: 10.1037/a0039260
  38. Hwang, M. J., & Jung, S. H. (2011). The ageing society of Korea and the population estimate. Korea Journal of Population Studies, 34(2), 113-133.
  39. Isreal, J. B., Chesney, G. L., Wickens, C. D., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and tracking difficulty: Evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. Psychophysiology, 17(3), 259-273. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb00146.x
  40. Jodo, E., & Kayama, Y. (1992). Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82(6), 477-482. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90122-x
  41. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 705. DOI: 10.1037/0021-843x.115.4.705
  42. Kim, S. Y., Jung, J. B., & Nam, K. C. (2018). Variability in the Visuo-spatial Attention Ability and Emotional Perception Ability Related with Bipolar Disorder Tendency in the Normal Pupulation. Science of Emotion & Sensibility, 21(2), 145-158. DOI: 10.14695/kjsos.2018.21.2.145
  43. Klein, M., Ponds, R. W., Houx, P. J., & Jolles, J. (1997). Effect of test duration on age-related differences in Stroop interference. Journal of Clinical and Experi-Mental Neuropsychology, 19(1), 77-82. DOI: 10.1080/01688639708403838
  44. Kosslyn, S. M., Brown, H. D., & Dror, I. E. (1999). Aging and the scope of visual attention. Gerontology, 45(2), 102-109. DOI: 10.1159/000022071
  45. LaBar, K. S., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (1999). Neuroanatomic overlap of working memory and spatial attention networks: a functional MRI comparison within subjects. Neuroimage, 10(6), 695-704. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0503
  46. Langley, L. K., Friesen, C. K., Saville, A. L., & Ciernia, A. T. (2011). Timing of reflexive visuospatial orienting in young, young-old, and old-old adults. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1546-1561. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-011-0108-8
  47. Lavie, N., & De Fockert, J. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 669-674. DOI: 10.3758/bf03196756
  48. Li, S. C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory (pp. 103-146). Hogrefe & Huber.
  49. Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikstrom, S. (2001). Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(11), 479-486. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01769-1
  50. Lucci, G., Berchicci, M., Spinelli, D., Taddei, F., & Di Russo, F. (2013). The effects of aging on conflict detection. PloS one, 8(2), e56566. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056566
  51. MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 598. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598
  52. Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: Tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 47(6), 532-550. DOI: 10.3758/bf03203106
  53. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14(5), 409-415. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455
  54. McCalley, L. T., Bouwhuis, D. G., & Juola, J. F. (1995). Age changes in the distribution of visual attention. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50(6), P316-P331. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/50B.6.P316
  55. McDowell, C. L., Harrison, D. W., & Demaree, H. A. (1994). Is right hemisphere decline in the perception of emotion a function of aging?. International Journal of Neuroscience, 79(1-2), 1-11. DOI: 10.3109/00207459408986063
  56. Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 542. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542
  57. Mishra, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Preserved discrimination performance and neural processing during crossmodal attention in aging. PLoS One, 8(11), e81894. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081894
  58. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial expressions presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition & Emotion, 13(6), 713-740. DOI: 10.1080/026999399379050
  59. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Damsma, E. (2000). Threat perception bias in nonreferred, socially anxious children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(3), 348-359. DOI: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2903_6
  60. Naghavi, H. R., & Nyberg, L. (2005). Common fronto-parietal activity in attention, memory, and consciousness: Shared demands on integration?. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(2), 390-425. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.003
  61. Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2009). Common and distinct neural correlates of perceptual and memorial selection. Neuroimage, 45(3), 963-975. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.005
  62. Nobre, A. C., & Kastner, S. (2014). The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.001.0001
  63. Novak, G., Ritter, W., & Vaughan Jr, H. G. (1992). Mismatch detection and the latency of temporal judgments. Psychophysiology, 29(4), 398-411. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb01713.x
  64. O'Connell, R. G., Balsters, J. H., Kilcullen, S. M., Campbell, W., Bokde, A. W., Lai, R., Upton, N., & Robertson, I. H. (2012). A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(10), 2448-2461. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021
  65. Onton, J., Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage, 27(2), 341-356. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014
  66. Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 75-92. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025
  67. Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299
  68. Petrican, R., Moscovitch, M., & Schimmack, U. (2008). Cognitive resources, valence, and memory retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23(3), 585. DOI: 10.1037/a0013176
  69. Phillips, L. H., MacLean, R. D., & Allen, R. (2002). Age and the understanding of emotions: Neuropsychological and sociocognitive perspectives. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57(6), 526-530. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/57.6.p526
  70. Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Champagne, S. C., & Nelson, R. F. (1984). The effects of age on human event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 21(3), 312-326. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb02941.x
  71. Potts, G. F. (2004). An ERP index of task relevance evaluation of visual stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 56(1), 5-13. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.03.006
  72. Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619-633. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhh023
  73. Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177-182. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x
  74. Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65(4), 405-415. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035
  75. Roberts, L. E., Rau, H., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1994). Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/No Go discrimination. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 92(1), 44-55. DOI: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90006-x
  76. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296-320. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0504_2
  77. Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(4), 863-881. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001
  78. Santesso, D. L., Meuret, A. E., Hofmann, S. G., Mueller, E. M., Ratner, K. G., Roesch, E. B., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Electrophysiological correlates of spatial orienting towards angry faces: A source localization study. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1338-1348. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.013
  79. Satler, C., & Tomaz, C. (2011). Emotional working memory in Alzheimer's disease patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 1(1), 124-138. DOI: 10.1155/2014/207698
  80. Seiple, W., Szlyk, J. P., Yang, S., & Holopigian, K. (1996). Age-related functional field losses are not eccentricity dependent. Vision Research, 36(12), 1859-1866. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00288-x
  81. Shimokawa, A., Yatomi, N., Anamizu, S., Torii, S., Isono, H., Sugai, Y., & Kohno, M. (2001). Influence of deteriorating ability of emotional comprehension on interpersonal behavior in Alzheimer-type dementia. Brain and Cognition, 47(3), 423-433. DOI: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1318
  82. Spell, L. A., & Frank, E. (2000). Recognition of nonverbal communication of affect following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(4), 285-300. DOI: 10.1023/a:1006675230193
  83. Staub, B., Doignon-Camus, N., Bacon, E., & Bonnefond, A. (2014). The effects of aging on sustained attention ability: An ERP study. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 684. DOI: 10.1037/a0037067
  84. Sullivan, S., & Ruffman, T. (2004). Emotion recognition deficits in the elderly. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114(3), 403-432. DOI: 10.1080/00207450490270901
  85. Surcinelli, P., Codispoti, M., Montebarocci, O., Rossi, N., & Baldaro, B. (2006). Facial emotion recognition in trait anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(1), 110-117. DOI: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.010
  86. Tales, A., Muir, J. L., Bayer, A., & Snowden, R. J. (2002). Spatial shifts in visual attention in normal ageing and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2000-2012. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00057-x
  87. Thomas, R. C., & Hasher, L. (2006). The influence of emotional valence on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 821. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.821
  88. Townsend, R. E., & Johnson, L. C. (1979). Relation of frequency-analyzed EEG to monitoring behavior. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47(3), 272-279. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(79)90279-7
  89. Vallesi, A., Stuss, D. T., McIntosh, A. R., & Picton, T. W. (2009). Age-related differences in processing irrelevant information: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 577-586. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.018
  90. Vallesi, A., & Stuss, D. T. (2010). Excessive subthreshold motor preparation for non-target stimuli in normal aging. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1251-1257. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022
  91. Vallesi, A. (2011). Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: A go/nogo event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 487(3), 313-317. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.046
  92. Vecera, S. P., & Rizzo, M. (2003). Spatial attention: Normal processes and their breakdown. Neurologic Clinics, 21(3), 575-607. DOI: 10.1016/s0733-8619(02)00103-2
  93. Wascher, E., Falkenstein, M., & Wild-Wall, N. (2011). Age related strategic differences in processing irrelevant information. Neuroscience Letters, 487(1), 66-69. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.075
  94. Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., & Gajewski, P. D. (2012). Neural correlates of changes in a visual search task due to cognitive training in seniors. Neural Plasticity, 529057. DOI: 10.1155/2012/529057
  95. Wong, B., Cronin-Golomb, A., & Neargarder, S. (2005). Patterns of visual scanning as predictors of emotion identification in normal aging. Neuropsychology, 19(6), 739. DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.739
  96. Zanto, T. P., Hennigan, K., Ostberg, M., Clapp, W. C., & Gazzaley, A. (2010). Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults. Cortex, 46(4), 564-574. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.003