DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Spatial Representation on the Part of Young Children according to Task Conditions

과제 제시방법에 따른 유아의 공간표상

  • 민미희 (서울대학교 아동가족학과 아동학전공) ;
  • 이순형 (서울대학교 아동가족학과)
  • Received : 2012.06.28
  • Accepted : 2012.10.05
  • Published : 2012.10.31

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of task conditions (physical similarity between the spatial product and the reference space, presentation place of the spatial product) on children's spatial representation. The participants consisted of 40 3-year-olds and 40 4-year-olds. The results of this study are as follows. Both 3-year-olds and 4-year-olds were capable of a greater degree of spatial representation when there was a high level of physical similarity between the spatial product and the reference space, and when the presentation place of the spatial product was in the reference space. 4-year-olds were capable of more accurate spatial representation than 3-year-olds. There was no significant difference in the children's spatial representation depending on the type of spatial product (scale model, map). The results revealed that the physical similarity between the spatial product and the reference space and the presentation place of the spatial product are essential in young children's spatial representation. Additionally, the results indicated that spatial representation of children develops gradually from when they are three to when they turn four.

Keywords

References

  1. Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1994). The development of children's ability to use spatial representations. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 25, pp. 157-199). San Diego : Academic Press.
  2. Bremner, J. G., & Andreasen, G. (1998). Young children's ability to use maps and models to find ways in novel spaces. British journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 197-218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00919.x
  3. Callaghan, T. C. (2000). Factors affecting graphic symbol understanding in the third year : Language, similarity and iconicity. Cognitive Development, 15, 207-236.
  4. Dalke, D. E. (1998). Charting the development of representational skills : When do children know that maps can lead and mislead? Cognitive Development, 13, 53-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90020-X
  5. DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children, Science, 238, 1556-1557. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2446392
  6. DeLoache, J. S. (1989). The Development of representation in young children. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 22, pp. 1-39). San Diego : Academic Press.
  7. DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children : Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62, 736-752. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131174
  8. DeLoache, J. S. (1995a). Early symbolic understanding and use. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 33, pp. 65-114). New York : Academic Press.
  9. DeLoache, J. S. (1995b). Early understanding and use of symbols : The model model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(4), 109-113. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772408
  10. DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and young children's use of scale model. Child Development, 71, 329-338. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00148
  11. DeLoache, J. S. (2002). Early development of the understanding and use of symbolic artifacts. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. pp. 206-226. Malden, MA : Blackwell.
  12. DeLoache, J. S., DeMendoza, O. A. P., & Anderson, K. N. (1999). Multiple factors in early symbol use : Instructions, similarity, and age in understanding a symbol-referent relation. Cognitive Development, 14, 299-312. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00006-4
  13. DeLoache, J. S., Kolstad, V., & Anderson, K. N. (1991). Physical similarity and young children's understanding of scale models. Child Development, 62, 111-126. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130708
  14. DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. W., & Pierroutsakos, S. (1998). Reasoning and problem-solving. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology : Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed, pp. 801-850). New York : Wiley.
  15. Downs, R. M., Liben, L. S., & Daggs, D. G. (1988). On education and geographers : The role of cognitive developmental theory in geographic education. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 78(4), 680-700. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8306.1988.tb00238.x
  16. Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (2003). Cognitive development (3rd ed.). Prentice Hall.
  17. Gentner, D. (1989). The mechanisms of analogical learning. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds)., Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 199-241). London : Cambridge University Press.
  18. Gentner, D., & Landers, R. (1985). Analogical reminding : A good match is hard to find. In S. Vosniadou, & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 245-267). New York : Cambridge University Press.
  19. Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Newcombe, N., & Duffy, S. (2008). Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time. Cognition, 106, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.006
  20. Jung, M. R. (1995). Development of spatial representation. Unpublished master's thesis, Kookmin University, Seoul, Korea.
  21. Liben, L. S. (1999). Developing an understanding of external spatial representation. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Development of mental representation-theories and applications.
  22. Liben, L. S., & Yekel, G. A. (1996). Preschoolers' understanding of plan and oblique maps : The role of geometric and representational correspondence. Child Development, 67, 780-2796.
  23. Loewenstein, J., & Gentner, D. (2001). Spatial mapping in preschoolers : Close comparisons facilitate far mappings. Journal of Cognition & Development, 2(2), 189-219. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0202_4
  24. Marzolf, D. P., & DeLoache, J. S. (1994). Transfer in young children's understanding of spatial representation. Child Development, 65, 1-15 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131361
  25. Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space : The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press.
  26. Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2006). Development of spatial cognition. D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.). Handbook of child psychology (16th edition, pp. 734-776). John Wiley and Sons.
  27. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. New York : Norton.
  28. Sharon, T., & DeLoache, J. S. (2003). The role of perseveration in children's symbolic understanding and skill. Developmental Science, 6(3), 289-296. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00285
  29. Sung, H. R., Yi, H. J., Kim, H. R., Park, Y. S., Park, S. M., Yu, Y. O., & Son, Y. S. (2001). Cognitive development. Seoul : Hakjisa.
  30. Troseth, G. L., Bloom Pickard, M. E., & DeLoache, J. S. (2007). Young children's use of scale models : testing an alternative to representational insight. Developmental Science, 10(6), 763-769. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00625.x
  31. Vasilyeva, M. & Bowers, E. (2010). Exploring the effects of similarity on mapping spatial relations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 221-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.003
  32. Vosmik, J. R. (2005). Facilitating preschooler's spatial mapping : Close comparisons and natural misalignment. Arizona State University doctoral dissertation.