• 제목/요약/키워드: the negation of negation

검색결과 98건 처리시간 0.022초

$3{\sim}6$세 아동의 대립어 습득 및 부정어 처리 발달 (The Development of Processing Negation and Opposites Acquisition from 3 to 6 Year-old Children)

  • 김형재
    • 한국지역사회생활과학회지
    • /
    • 제19권4호
    • /
    • pp.569-580
    • /
    • 2008
  • The purpose of this study is to investigate opposites acquisition and tendency of processing negations about directional oppositionality from 3- to 6-old Korean children. The data were collected from 80 children from a daycare center and 20 university students in Busan, Korea. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to produce opposites for common terms related to colors, directions, and dimension adjectives. In Experiment 2, they were asked to make pictures corresponding to statements with negations(e. g. The arrow is NOT pointing up.). Summarizing the overall results, first, children over 5 considered 'blue' and 'red' as opposites colors in the case of not well-known color opposites. If there exists an opposite color, color opposites have been acquired from 6 year-olds, the directional opposites started from 3 year-olds, and the opposites of dimension adjectives did from 5 year-olds. Second, preferring the antipodal color opposites started from 6 and preferring the antipodal direction did from 3. Consequently, 3-to 6 year-old children acquired opposites at different age and opposites dimension. Also the opposites dimension have an effect on processing negation of young children.

  • PDF

부정 스키마의 의미론적 양상 (Semantic Aspects of Negation as Schema)

  • 태강수
    • 정보처리학회논문지B
    • /
    • 제9B권1호
    • /
    • pp.23-28
    • /
    • 2002
  • 지능형 에이전트를 구현하는데 있어서 하나의 근본적인 문제는 에이전트가 자신의 인식이나 행동의 의미를 이해하지 못한다는 점이다. 에이전트가 세계를 이해하지 못하는 이유중의 하나는 의미론적 자질을 단순한 문자열로 변환시키는 구문론적 접근방법에서 야기한다. 이 문제를 해결하기 위해 코헨은 에이전트가 자율적으로 자신의 센서와 행동자를 사용하여 환경과 상호작용 함으로써 고급 개념의 기초가 되는 물리적 스키마를 배우는 의미론적 방법을 소개한다. 하지만 코헨은 스키마를 이해하는 것을 가능하게 해주는 상위 계층의 개념소자는 다루지 않는다. 본 논문에서는 부정은 물리적 스키마의 인식을 가능하게 해주는 메타 스키마라는 제안을 하고 부정의 몇 가지 의미론적 양상들을 증명한다.

On the Scope of Negation in Japanese

  • Yukinori, Takubo
    • 한국언어정보학회:학술대회논문집
    • /
    • 한국언어정보학회 1983년도 Seoul Papers in Formal Garmmar Theory = Proceedings of the Third Korean-Japanese Joint Workshop
    • /
    • pp.35-46
    • /
    • 1983
  • PDF

Negation and Compositionality

  • Lee, Hwan-Mook
    • 한국언어정보학회:학술대회논문집
    • /
    • 한국언어정보학회 1985년도 Proceedings of 84 Matsuyama Workshop on Formal Grammar
    • /
    • pp.95-105
    • /
    • 1985
  • PDF

One-sided Readings of Numbers in Modal Sentences

  • Kwak, Eun-Joo
    • 영어영문학
    • /
    • 제57권3호
    • /
    • pp.429-455
    • /
    • 2011
  • Numbers have been regarded as one-sided, and their exactly readings have been understood as the results of scalar implicature. This Neo-Gricean view on numbers becomes less persuasive due to theoretical and experimental counterarguments. In spite of growing evidence for theirtwo-sided readings, numbers are still one-sided in modal sentences. Moreover, the occurrence of a negative operator may worsen the acceptability of modal sentences with numbers. In the framework of Vector Space Semantics, I have derived two-sided readings of numbers with the simple notions of monotonicity of modals and scopal relations between modals and numbers. I have also argued that the awkwardness incurred by negation is the result of a split set of vectors for a number. The incoherent set of vectors is understood as the lack of an ideal behavior, which is against the deontic modality of the sentence.

억양을 통한 부정문의 중의성 해소 방안 연구 (Disambiguation of Negative Sentences by Intonation)

  • 김소희;공은정;강선미;이용재
    • 음성과학
    • /
    • 제7권4호
    • /
    • pp.187-202
    • /
    • 2000
  • The negative sentence may have an ambiguity depending on which constituent of the sentence is negated. In case of sentence final adverbials, whether they are included in the scope of negation generates the semantic ambiguity. Since sentences with ambiguous meanings have the same word order, the differences of the meanings in different contexts should be manifested with intonational cues. This article represents how intonation contributes to the disambiguation in negative sentences with ambiguity and which phonological/phonetic cues are specifically used in the course of the disambiguation.

  • PDF

Modality and Modal Sense Representation in E-HowNet

  • Chung, You-Shan;Huang, Shu-Ling;Chen, Keh-Jiann
    • 한국언어정보학회:학술대회논문집
    • /
    • 한국언어정보학회 2007년도 정기학술대회
    • /
    • pp.136-145
    • /
    • 2007
  • This paper explains how we define and represent modality in E-HowNet. Following Lyons (1977, reviewed in Hsieh 2003, among others), we hold that modals express a speaker's opinion or attitude toward a proposition and hence have a pragmatic dimension and recognize five kinds of modal categories, i.e. epistemic, deontic, ability, volition and expectation modality. We then present a representational formalism that contains the three most basic components of modal meaning: modal category, positive or negative and strength. Such a formula can define not only modal words but also words that contain modal meanings and cope with co-compositions of modals and the negation construction.

  • PDF

한국 아동의 부정사 획득 (The Acquisition of Negatives in Five Korean Children)

  • 이순형
    • 아동학회지
    • /
    • 제6권1호
    • /
    • pp.17-40
    • /
    • 1985
  • This study investigated Korean children's early acquisition of negatives and focused on four research questions: 1) processing of negative variations; 2) the nature of negatives when negatives are completely acquired in Korean (in which meaning and form are matched in one to one mapping); 3) the validity of Bellugi's negative acquisition model in Korean; and 4) the cause of child's erroneous sentence production: limited ability or regularity in children's cognition. The language data of the five subjects (age span; 1.1 - 3.11) were collected by their parents in the natural setting of the home. The results showed that 1) the pivot form, was processed in many ways from a simple to a complicated form, such as <(X+X')+N> <(x+x')+N,Y> <(x+x') N,(y+y')>. It appeared that the children used a simple negative format to reach a one-step advanced negative format. 2) Korean negatives are divided into range of negation in the negative sentence (part or whole), strength of negation (absolute or general), functions of meaning (negation, absences, refusal, prohibition, impossibility). All five children acquired negative sentences in all functions and the complete range after 3 years of age. 3) In spite of the differences in age level, Bellugi's four stage model was in evidence; that is, Korean children's negative acquisition was almost identical with Bellugi's tour stage model in deep structure. 4) Analyses of children's error sentences showed that the sentences with errors were made not because of the children's limitation in cognitive ability but because of the strict application of regularity of rules from the original grammars. Consequently, the children produced negative sentences using two rules: the rule of additive complexity (from simple to complex) and the rule of division (from one to several).

  • PDF