• Title/Summary/Keyword: 중국어

Search Result 250, Processing Time 0.022 seconds

On the 'realization' meaning of possibility expressions - '-ul swu iss-' and its counterparts in Japanese and Chinese - (가능 표현의 실현 용법에 대하여 - '-을 수 있-' 및 일본어·중국어의 대응 표현을 중심으로 -)

  • Kang, Yeongri;Xu, Cuie;Park, Jinho
    • Cross-Cultural Studies
    • /
    • v.50
    • /
    • pp.313-346
    • /
    • 2018
  • It is noted that generally speaking, the expressing of actualization or non-actualization of events is not the main role of possibility for the utilization of expressions. In spite of this fact, it is possible to see many examples in which possibility expressions represent actual events, and impossibility expressions represent a type of non-actualization in relation to events. This effect can be described as a semantic extension, by which the participant-internal possibility is extended to actualization due to participant-internal factors, and the participant-external possibility is extended to the actualization due to participant-external factors. When the related possibility expressions are used in this extended sense, they express the dynamic evaluative meaning of 'desirability' of the realized event, while it is determined that when the impossibility expressions are used in this extended sense, they are seen to express the evaluative meaning of 'regretfulness' about the non-actualization of the event. In Modern Japanese, it is noted that there are a few expressions of ability and possibility. They can be largely divided into four types of expressions, according to their origins or uses of expression, which are 'ability verbs', affixes '-れる/られる(-reru/rareru)', '-できる(-dekiru)', and '-得る(-eru)'. They can all express participant-internal possibility and participant-external non-deontic possibility. While 'ability verbs', affixes '-れる/られる' and '-できる' can express participant-external deontic possibility, '-得る' cannot. However, '-得る' is the only possible element to designate the event of a epistemic possibility. Also, the four types of expressions have the usage of conveying 'actualization/non-actualization,' as is the case of the Korean language. However in Japanese, in fact adjectives cannot be associated with 'ability verbs' or 'ability affixes.' Thus the expressions of 'regrets' should in that case depend on the use contexts, unlike the expression 'adj+-지 못하다' as noted in Korean. The ability and possibility in Modern Chinese are mainly expressed by means of the four auxiliary verbs '能($n{\acute{e}}ng$)', '会(huì)', '可以(kěyǐ)' and '可能 ($k{\check{e}}n{\acute{e}}ng$)'. '能' and '会' along with '可以' can all convey participant-internal possibility. In this way '能' and '可以' can express participant-external possibility. Only '会' and '可能' can express epistemic possibility. As for 'actualization,' among the four auxiliary verbs, only '能' can represent actualization. Also, among the negatives of the four auxiliary verbs, only '沒能' can represent non-actualization.

A Method of Analyzing Sentiment Polarity of Multilingual Social Media: A Case of Korean-Chinese Languages (다국어 소셜미디어에 대한 감성분석 방법 개발: 한국어-중국어를 중심으로)

  • Cui, Meina;Jin, Yoonsun;Kwon, Ohbyung
    • Journal of Intelligence and Information Systems
    • /
    • v.22 no.3
    • /
    • pp.91-111
    • /
    • 2016
  • It is crucial for the social media based marketing practices to perform sentiment analyze the unstructured data written by the potential consumers of their products and services. In particular, when it comes to the companies which are interested in global business, the companies must collect and analyze the data from the social media of multinational settings (e.g. Youtube, Instagram, etc.). In this case, since the texts are multilingual, they usually translate the sentences into a certain target language before conducting sentiment analysis. However, due to the lack of cultural differences and highly qualified data dictionary, translated sentences suffer from misunderstanding the true meaning. These result in decreasing the quality of sentiment analysis. Hence, this study aims to propose a method to perform a multilingual sentiment analysis, focusing on Korean-Chinese cases, while avoiding language translations. To show the feasibility of the idea proposed in this paper, we compare the performance of the proposed method with those of the legacy methods which adopt language translators. The results suggest that our method outperforms in terms of RMSE, and can be applied by the global business institutions.

The International Comparative Study on the Origin of the Terms of Mineral and Rock (광물·암석 용어의 어원에 대한 국제 비교 연구)

  • Lee, Chang-Zin;Ryu, Chun-Ryol;Cho, Jun-Oh
    • Journal of the Korean earth science society
    • /
    • v.32 no.3
    • /
    • pp.306-323
    • /
    • 2011
  • The purpose of this study is to analyse the origin of the terms for well-known minerals and rocks in South Korean, North Korean, English, Chinese and Japanese. All of the terms for mineral and rock have been standardized in English and Chinese, Whereas not all of the terms are standardized yet in Japanese, North Korean and South Korean. Although many Korean researchers have recognized the fact and tried to standardize the terms, most of the terms for minerals and rocks have been used without any research conducted on them, which made it impossible. The terms for minerals were mainly made based on some of 28 geologic properties: chemical composition, color, shape and material. The terms for rocks were mainly originated from some of their 30 geologic properties: mineral composition, origin of rock, material and surface. 23 of the 124 English terms were named after the names of men or location, Whereas only 3 terms were originated from a name of place in the Eastern countries. In the Eastern countries, the terms for some minerals or rocks often consist of more complex geologic properties, compared with English terms for the same ones. For the minerals with complex geological properties, in the Eastern countries, there are 28 terms on average, which is much more than 2 in English. There are 9.25 terms in average for the rocks with complex geological properties in the Eastern while only 5 terms exist in English. Some of the Korean terms are very difficult for students to recognize what they are because the terms were originated from Japanese or English terms, which were translated into Korean without consideration of Korean contexts. Therefore the terms of rocks and minerals need to be discussed about their meaning and relevance.

Perception of Korean Vowels by English and Mandarin Learners of Korean: Effects of Acoustic Similarity Between L1 and L2 Sounds and L2 Experience (영어권, 중국어권 학습자의 한국어 모음 지각 -모국어와 목표 언어 간의 음향 자질의 유사성과 한국어 경험의 효과 중심으로-)

  • Ryu, Na-Young
    • Journal of Korean language education
    • /
    • v.29 no.1
    • /
    • pp.1-23
    • /
    • 2018
  • This paper investigates how adult Mandarin- and English- speaking learners of Korean perceive Korean vowels, with focus on the effect of the first language (L1) and the second language (L2) acoustic relationship, as well as the influence of Korean language experience. For this study, native Mandarin and Canadian English speakers who have learned Korean as a foreign language, as well as a control group of native Korean speakers, participated in two experiments. Experiment 1 was designed to examine acoustic similarities between Korean and English vowels, as well as Korean and Mandarin vowels to predict which Korean vowels are relatively easy, or difficult for L2 learners to perceive. The linear discriminant analysis (Klecka, 1980) based on their L1-L2 acoustic similarity predicted that L2 Mandarin learners would have perceptual difficulty rankings for Korean vowels as follows: (the easiest) /i, a, e/ >> /ɨ, ʌ, o, u/ (most difficult), whereas L2 English learners would have perceptual difficulty rankings for Korean vowels as follows: (the easiest) /i, a, e, ɨ, ʌ/ >> /o, u/ (most difficult). The goal of Experiment 2 was to test how accurately L2 Mandarin and English learners perceive Korean vowels /ɨ, ʌ, o, u/ which are considered to be difficult for L2 learners. The results of a mixed-effects logistic model revealed that English listeners showed higher identification accuracy for Korean vowels than Mandarin listeners, indicating that having a larger L1 vowel inventory than the L2 facilitates L2 vowel perception. However, both groups have the same ranking of Korean vowel perceptual difficulty: ɨ > ʌ > u > o. This finding indicates that adult learners of Korean can perceive the new vowel /ɨ/, which does not exist in their L1, more accurately than the vowel /o/, which is acoustically similar to vowels in their L1, suggesting that L2 learners are more likely to establish additional phonetic categories for new vowels. In terms of the influence of experience with L2, it was found that identification accuracy increases as Korean language experience rises. In other words, the more experienced English and Mandarin learners of Korean are, the more likely they are to have better identification accuracy in Korean vowels than less experienced learners of Korean. Moreover, there is no interaction between L1 background and L2 experience, showing that identification accuracy of Korean vowels is higher as Korean language experience increases regardless of their L1 background. Overall, these findings of the two experiments demonstrated that acoustic similarity between L1 and L2 sounds using the LDA model can partially predict perceptual difficulty in L2 acquisition, indicating that other factors such as perceptual similarity between L1 and L2, the merge of Korean /o/ and /u/ may also influence their Korean vowel perception.

Learning acoustic cue weights for Korean stops through L2 perception training (지각 훈련을 통한 한국어 폐쇄음 음향 신호 가중치의 L2 학습)

  • Oh, Eunjin
    • Phonetics and Speech Sciences
    • /
    • v.13 no.4
    • /
    • pp.9-21
    • /
    • 2021
  • This study investigated whether Korean learners improve acoustic cue weights to identify Korean lenis and aspirated stops in the direction of native values through perception training that focused on contrasting the stops in various phonetic contexts. Nineteen native Chinese learners of Korean and two native Korean instructors for the perception training participated in the experiment. A training group and a non-training group were divided according to pretest results, and only the training group participated in the training for 5 days. To estimate the perceptual weights of the stop cues, a pretest and a posttest were conducted with stimuli whose stop cues (F0 and VOT) were systematically manipulated. Binary logistic regression analyses were performed on each learner's test results to calculate perceptual β coefficients, which estimate the perceptual weights of the acoustic cues used in identifying the stop contrast. The training group showed a statistically significant increase of 0.451 on average in the posttest for the coefficient values of the F0, which is the primary cue for the stop contrast, whereas the non-training group showed an insignificant increase of 0.246. The patterns of change in the F0 use after training varied considerably among individual learners.

A Study on the Factors Influencing the Acceptance of K-pop Short-form Video Created by Chinese Influencers - Focusing on Chinese TikTok Users (중국 인플루언서들의 K-pop 짧은 동영상 수용에 영향을 미치는 요인에 관한 연구 - 중국 '틱톡' 사용자를 중심으로)

  • Liu, QuanQuan;Yu, Sae-Kyung
    • The Journal of the Korea Contents Association
    • /
    • v.22 no.4
    • /
    • pp.28-36
    • /
    • 2022
  • This study analyzed 284 K-pop song and dance cover short-form videos recreated by Chinese influencers uploaded on TikTok, to explore which reform factors of image similarity, language similarity, the extent of audience participation leading, the extent of lyrics or subtitles translated into Chinese, PPL disclosure, the length of video and the reputation of influencer affected Chinese TikTok audiences' reactions - number of "Likes," "Comments" and "Shares." The results showed that only the "reputation of influencer" was significantly affected the number of "Likes" which estimated as a relatively passive response, but the other factors affected the number of "Comments" and "Shares" significantly which estimated as more active responses. The more an influencer is perceived as not similar to the singer in terms of image the more comments were posted. And the videos expressed in Korean archived more comments and shares than those lyrics or subtitles translated into Chinese. This study is meaningful in that it confirmed the necessity of influencers in the globe diffusion of K-pop, by specifically analyzing the audience's reactions according to the characteristics of UCCs created by local influencers using short-form video platforms.

Cognitive Individual Differences and L2 Learners' Processing of Korean Subject-Object Relative Clauses (인지능력의 개별차와 한국어 학습자의 주격-목적격 관계절 프로세싱)

  • Goo, Jaemyung
    • Asia-pacific Journal of Multimedia Services Convergent with Art, Humanities, and Sociology
    • /
    • v.8 no.6
    • /
    • pp.493-503
    • /
    • 2018
  • The present study is a conceptual replication of O'Grady, Lee, and Choo's (2003) earlier study designed to investigate two hypotheses (linear distance hypothesis vs. structural distance hypothesis) in relation to L2 Korean learners' processing of Korean subject and object relative clauses (RCs) in a scholarly attempt to explicate Keenan and Comrie's (1977) Noun Phrase Accessibility Hierarchy (NPAH). In addition, the current study is intended to explore any potential role of working memory capacity (WMC) in the processing of Korean subject and/or object RCs. Chinese-speaking learners of Korean taking a language course offered at a local university in Korea participated in this experimental study. Among those recruited, only 23 learners completed the experimental tasks appropriately according to the specific instructions provided on each task, and thus, subsequent statistical analyses were conducted on their data. Fifteen Korean NSs were also recruited for the control group. Two experimental tasks were administerd to the participants: one picture selection task containing the same test items used in O'Grady et al.'s study to measure their processing of subject-object RCs and an operation span (OSPAN) task to measure their WMC. Somewhat differently from O'Grady et al.'s findings, the participating Chinese learners of Korean performed significantly better on object RCs than on subject RCs, seemingly lending support to the linear distance hypothesis. Further analyses, however, suggested that the results in favor of, or relative ease of processing, object relative clauses were due, most likely, to the learners' excessive use of the canonical sentence strategy, which also led to nonsignificant correlations between WMC and learner performance on the picture selection task.

Automatic Back-Transliteration with Word Origin Information (어원 정보를 이용한 외래어의 자동 원어 복원)

  • Lee, Sang-Yool;Kang, In-Su;Na, Seung-Hoon;Lee, Jong-Hyeok
    • Annual Conference on Human and Language Technology
    • /
    • 2003.10d
    • /
    • pp.54-60
    • /
    • 2003
  • 음차 표기된 외래어로부터 원어를 복원하는 문제는 원어의 발음정보를 이용한 통계적인 방법을 많이 사용한다. 하지만 지금까지의 연구들은 대부분 영어단어만을 그 대상으로 하였기 때문에 '도쿄(Tokyo)', '하인리히(Hinrich)'와 같이 어원이 영어가 아닌 단어들의 복원에는 좋은 결과를 보여주지 못했다. 이러한 문제를 해결하기 위하여 한글로 표기된 외래어의 어원을 판단할 수 있는 방법을 찾아내고, 이 방법을 통해 외래어를 어원별로 분리하여 학습모델을 구축함으로써 다양한 어원을 가진 외래어들의 복원 정확률을 높이고자 하였다. 위의 방식으로 구현된 시스템은 영어, 일본어, 중국어, 프랑스어의, 서로 다른 4개의 어원을 가진 데이터의 복원 실험에서 기존의 방식에 비해 13%의 성능 향상을 보였다.

  • PDF

The Influence of Chinese Falling-rising Tone on the Pitch of Sino-Korean Words Pronounced by Chinese Learners: Focusing on Same-form-same-meaning Words (중국인의 한국어 한자어 발음에서 보이는 중국어 상성의 영향: 동형동의어를 중심으로)

  • Kim, Young-Joo;Liu, Si-Yang
    • Phonetics and Speech Sciences
    • /
    • v.3 no.2
    • /
    • pp.11-22
    • /
    • 2011
  • The purpose of this study is to find the influence of Chinese falling-rising tone on the pitch pattern of corresponding Sino-Korean words delivered by Chinese learners of Korean and to examine how the falling-rising tone of corresponding Chinese words affect the pitch patterns of Sino-Korean words. The scope of this research is limited to Chinese learners of Korean, especially when they pronounce same-form-same-meaning Sino-Korean words. In this study, Chinese learners pronounced both Chinese words and corresponding Sino-Korean words. Learners' pitch patterns were recorded and analyzed using software and compared with the tone of corresponding Chinese words. Experimental results showed that Sino-Korean words were affected by Chinese 'falling-rising tone - high and level tone' when they started with lenis sounds. On the other hand, when Sino-Korean words started with aspirated sounds they were affected by Chinese 'falling-rising tone - high and level tone', 'falling-rising tone - falling-rising tone', and 'falling-rising tone - falling tone'. In conclusion, the Chinese learners' pitch patterns of Sino-Korean words are affected by Chinese falling-rising tone, especially when Sino-Korean words start with aspirated sounds.

  • PDF

French-Korean Computer-Assisted Translation Workbench, TransFranCo (불-한 전문분야 기계보조번역 워크벤치 TransFranCo)

  • Jeong, Hwi-Woong;Lim, Yong-Seok;Yoon, Ae-Sun
    • Proceedings of the Korean Society for Cognitive Science Conference
    • /
    • 2005.05a
    • /
    • pp.255-260
    • /
    • 2005
  • 번역 메모리(Translation Memory)는 오늘날 기계번역에 있어 통계기반 접근법이나 형태-통사적 접근법 모두에 있어 가장 중요한 요소로 평가되고 있다. 그러나 번역 메모리는 언어의 자질 및 각 용례를 통합적으로 관리해야 하며, 이를 기계가 자동으로 처리해주어야 하는 어려움이 있다. 최근에는 이러한 문제점을 해결하기 위해 다국적 기업을 중심으로 기계보조번역(Computer Aided Translation) 환경에 대한 연구가 활발히 이루어지고 있으나, 언어적인 특성 보다는 번역 메모리의 저장/대치적 측면에서 주요 연구가 이루어지고 있다. 이 논문에서는 번역 메모리 정보가 보다 높은 재사용성을 보이기 위해서는 다양한 언어자질값을 담을 수 있어야 한다고 보고, 이를 효율적으로 관리/구축할 수 있는 기계보조번역 워크벤치의 framework을 제시한다. 언어분석을 위한 대상언어로는 교역 및 기술 측면에서 영어, 일어, 중국어 다음으로 영향력이 높은 불어를 채택하며, 기존 기계보조번역 방식에 대한 고찰을 통해 개선된 번역 메모리 관리, 자동분석/번역 모듈 및 협업(collaboration) 방안에 대해 소개하고, 향후 발전방향에 대해 논의한다.

  • PDF