1 |
Port, R., Dalby, J., & O'Dell, M. (1987). Evidence for mora timing in Japanese. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81(5), 1574-1585.
DOI
|
2 |
Saha, S., & Mandal, S. (2018). Phonetic realization of English lexical stress by native (L1) Bengali speakers compared to native (L1) English speakers. Computer Speech & Language, 47, 1-15.
|
3 |
Sluijter, A., & Van Heuven, V. (1996). Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(4), 2471-2485.
DOI
|
4 |
Tono, Y., Kawaguchi, Y., & Minegishi, M. (2012). Developmental and crosslinguistic perspectives in learner corpus research (Vol. 4). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.
|
5 |
Tsujimura, N. (1996). An introduction to Japanese linguistics. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.
|
6 |
Tsushima, T. (2015). A case study: How do Japanese learners of English learn to use acoustic cues to differentiate stressed and unstressed syllables in pronunciation training?. The Journal of Humanities and Natural Sciences, 137, 33-57.
|
7 |
Whalen, D., & Xu, Y. (1992). Information for Mandarin tones in the amplitude contour and in brief segments. Phonetica, 49(1), 25-47.
DOI
|
8 |
Zhang, Y., Nissen, S., & Francis, A. (2008). Acoustic characteristics of English lexical stress produced by native Mandarin speakers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(6), 4498-4513.
DOI
|
9 |
Anderson-Hsieh, J., Johnson, R., & Koehler, K. (1992). The relationship between native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmentals, prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning, 42(4), 529-555.
DOI
|
10 |
Zuraiq, W., & Sereno, J. (2007). English lexical stress cues in native English and non-native Arabic speakers. Ratio, 1(2), 829-832.
|
11 |
Beckman, M., & Pierrehumbert, J. (1986). Intonational structure in Japanese and English. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 255-309.
|
12 |
Archibald, J. (1992). Transfer of L1 parameter settings: Some empirical evidence from polish metrics. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 37(3), 301-340.
|
13 |
Beckman, M. (1982). Segment duration and the 'mora' in Japanese. Phonetica, 39(2-3), 113-135.
DOI
|
14 |
Beckman, M. (1986). Stress and non-stress accent. Dordrecht: Walter de Gruyter (Vol. 7).
|
15 |
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 6.0.21. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/ on January 16, 2018.
|
16 |
Bolinger, D. (1958). A theory of pitch accent in English. Word, 14(2-3), 109-149.
DOI
|
17 |
Cheng, R. (1985). A comparison of Taiwanese, Taiwan Mandarin, and Peking Mandarin. Language, 352-377.
|
18 |
Elder, C., Golombek, P. Nguyen, T., & Ingram, J. (2005). Vietnamese acquisition of English word stress. TESOL Quarterly, 39(2), 309-319.
|
19 |
Fokes, J., & Bond, Z. (1989). The vowels of stressed and unstressed syllables in nonnative English. Language Learning, 39(3), 341-373.
DOI
|
20 |
Flege, J., & Bohn, O. (1989). An instrumental study of vowel reduction and stress placement in Spanish-accented English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11(1), 35-62.
|
21 |
Fry, D. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(4), 765-768.
DOI
|
22 |
Guion, S. (2005). Knowledge of English word stress patterns in early and late Korean-English bilinguals. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(4), 503-533.
DOI
|
23 |
Kondo, M. (2009). Is acquisition of L2 phonemes difficult? Production of English stress by Japanese speakers. Proceedings of the 10th Generative approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2009) (pp. 105-112). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings. Project, 2009.
|
24 |
Hong, S. (2012). A phonetic study on the production and training of English lexical stress by Korean speakers. The Journal of Modern British & American Language & Literature, 30(4), 99-122.
|
25 |
Kang, O. (2010). Relative salience of suprasegmental features on judgments of L2 comprehensibility and accentedness. System, 38(2), 301-315.
DOI
|
26 |
Kim, H., & Han, J. (1998). Vowel length in modern Korean: An acoustic analysis. In B. Park, & J. Yoon (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Korean Linguistics (pp. 412-418). Seoul: Hanshin.
|
27 |
Liu, S., & Samuel, A. (2004). Perception of Mandarin lexical tones when F0 information is neutralized. Language and Speech, 47(2), 109-138.
DOI
|
28 |
Kondo, M., Tsubaki, H., & Sagisaka, Y. (2015). Segmental variation of Japanese speakers' English: Analysis of "the North Wind and the Sun" in AESOP Corpus (< Feature Articles> Corpus-Based Data-Driven Speech Research in Asia). Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 19(1), 3-17.
|
29 |
Kwon, H. (2007). A phonetic analysis of advanced Korean EFL Learners' English reduced vowels. Korean Journal of English Language and Literature, 7(4), 547-564.
|
30 |
Lee, B., Guion, S., & Harada, T. (2006). Acoustic analysis of the production of unstressed English vowels by early and late Korean and Japanese bilinguals. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(3), 487-513.
DOI
|
31 |
McAllister, R., Flege, J., & Piske, T. (2002). The influence of L1 on the acquisition of Swedish quantity by native speakers of Spanish, English and Estonian. Journal of Phonetics, 30(2), 229-258.
DOI
|
32 |
Peterson, G., & Lehiste, I. (1960). Duration of syllable nuclei in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 32(6), 693-703.
DOI
|
33 |
Moore, C., & Jongman, A. (1997). Speaker normalization in the perception of Mandarin Chinese tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(3), 1864-1877.
DOI
|
34 |
Na, T. (2013). Comparison of acoustic characteristics of English lexical stress between Korean and Mandarin speakers. (Unpublished Master's Thesis). Yonsei University. Retrieved from RISS on January 20, 2018.
|
35 |
Ou, S. (2010). Taiwanese EFL learners. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics, 36(1), 1-23.
|