DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Autobiographical Memory of Childhood and Prosocial Behaviors

나는 순수했다!: 아동기에 대한 자서전적 기억과 친사회적 행동

  • 신홍임 (순천대학교 자유전공학부)
  • Received : 2020.09.15
  • Accepted : 2020.12.21
  • Published : 2021.03.31

Abstract

Previous studies have demonstrated that childhood memories can impact self-concepts. However, scant research has been conducted to determine whether and how the activation of childhood memories relates to the motivation of prosocial behaviors. Thus, this study investigated whether childhood memories facilitated prosocial behaviors through implicitly activating moral purity and how differently an abstract (vs. a concrete) construal level of autobiographical memories evoke prosocial intentions. According to the results of Study 1, the participants in the experimental condition of childhood memories were more motivated to perform prosocial behaviors than those in the controlled condition of recalling recent mundane activities. In the experimental condition, moral purity was activated more strongly than in the control condition. Study 2 demonstrated that participants in the "concrete" condition of childhood memories tended to the lower levels of prosocial motivation than those in the "abstract" condition wherein they were counting and describing good deeds from their childhood in detail. These results indicate that different construal levels (abstract vs. concrete) can mediate the relationship between childhood memories and prosocial behaviors. This study contributes to extending previous research regarding the determinants of motivating prosocial behaviors in cognitive processes.

지금까지 선행연구에서는 아동기에 대한 자서전적 기억이 한 개인의 자기개념에 영향을 끼침을 보고하였다. 반면, 아동기 경험의 회상방식이 성인기의 친사회적 행동에 어떻게 영향을 끼치는지에 대한 연구는 거의 수행되지 않았다. 이에 따라 본 연구에서는 아동기에 대한 자서전적 기억과 친사회적 행동의 관계를 해석수준에 따라 분석하려 한다. 연구 1에서는 대학생들을 대상으로 아동기 또는 최근 일상의 경험을 회상하는 조건에서 다양한 친사회적 행동에 대한 동기화가 달라지는지를 비교하였다. 연구 2에서는 아동기 경험을 추상적 또는 구체적으로 회상하는 방식이 해석수준의 차이를 통해 친사회적 행동에 영향을 끼치는지를 검증하였다. 그 결과 연구 1에서는 아동기의 경험을 회상하는 조건에서 친사회적 행동의 동기화정도가 높게 나타났다. 또한 연구참가자들은 아동기의 경험을 회상하는 조건에서 최근 일상을 회상하는 조건보다 자신의 도덕적 순수함에 대해 더 높게 평정하였다. 연구 2에서는 아동기의 도움행동을 구체적으로 회상하는 조건에서 아동기의 경험을 추상적으로 회상하는 조건보다 향후 친사회적 행동의 동기화가 감소됨을 보여주었다. 이 결과는 아동기의 경험에 대한 해석수준(추상적 vs. 구체적)과 도덕적 면허의 감정이 친사회적 행동의 동기화에 영향을 끼칠 가능성을 보여준다. 본 연구는 한 개인의 친사회적 행동과 연관된 인지과정의 변인을 탐색하여 선행연구를 확장했다는데 의의가 있다.

Keywords

References

  1. Aknin, L. B., Van de Vondervoort, J. W., & Hamlin, J. K. (2018). Positive feelings reward and promote prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 55-59. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.017
  2. Alicke, M. D., & Sedikides, C. (2009). Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and what they do. European Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 1-48. DOI: 10.1080/10463280802613866
  3. Agerstrom, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2009). Moral concerns are greater for temporally distant events and are moderated by value strength. Social Cognition, 27, 261-282. DOI: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.2.261
  4. Aydinli, A., Bender, M., Chasiotis, A. et al. (2014). When does self-reported prosocial motivation predict helping? The moderating role of implicit prosocial motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 645-658. DOI: 10.1007/s11031-014-9411-8
  5. Baddeley, A. (1995). Working memory. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 755-764). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  6. Barlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  7. Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automacity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype priming on action. Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230-244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
  8. Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Troschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1014-1027. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.1014
  9. Brewer, W. F. (1986). What is autobiographical memory? In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 25-49). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9780511558313.006
  10. Conway, M. A. (1990). Autobiographical memory: An introduction. Buckingham, England: Open University Press.
  11. Conway, M. A., & Rubin, D. C. (1993). The structure of autobiographical memory. In A. E. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. M. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 103-137). Hove, England: Erlbaum.
  12. Conway, P., & Peetz J. (2012). When does feeling moral actually make you a better person? Conceptual abstraction moderates whether past moral deeds motivate consistency or compensatory behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(7), 907-919. DOI: 10.1177/0146167212442394
  13. Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329(5987), 47-50. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1188595
  14. Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perceptionbehavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology 33 (pp. 1-40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80003-4
  15. Einolf, C. J. (2008). Empathic concern and prosocial behaviors: A test of experimental results using survey data. Social Science Research, 37(4), 1267-1279. DOI: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.06.003
  16. Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others' self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 739-752. DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.00
  17. Fujita, K., & Han, H. A. (2009). Moving beyond deliberative control of impulses: The effect of construal levels on evaluative associations in selfcontrol conflicts. Psychological Science, 20(7), 799-804. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x
  18. Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 351-367. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351
  19. Fazio, R. H., Effrein, E. A., & Falender, V. J. (1981). Self-perception following social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(2), 232-242. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.232
  20. Gino, F., & S. Desai. (2012). Memory lane and morality: How childhood memories promote prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(4), 743-758. DOI: 10.1037/a0026565
  21. Grant, A., & Dutton, J. (2012). Beneficiary or benefactor: Are people more prosocial when they reflect on receiving or giving? Psychological Science, 23(9), 1033-1039. DOI: 10.1177/0956797612439424
  22. Jones, E. E., Rhodewalt, E., Berglas, S., & Skelton, J. A. (1981). Effects of strategic self-presentation on subsequent self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(3), 407-421. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.3.407
  23. James, A., Jenks, C., & Prout, A. (1998). Theorizing childhood. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  24. Kim , T. G. (2020). 한겨레 아카이브 프로젝트. 시간의 극장. 한겨레신문. Retrieved from http://www.hani.co.kr/arti/politics/politics_general/954252.html
  25. Ko, H., & Kim, S. (2019). Factors affecting intentions of reporting child abuse: The interaction between type of abuse, sex, and construal-level. Science of Emotion & Sensibility, 22(2), 3-16. DOI: 10.14695/KJSOS.2018.22.2.3
  26. Lee, H. H., Kim, E. J., & Lee, M. K. (2003). A validation study of Korea positive and negative affect schedule: The PANAS scales. The Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22(4), 935-946.
  27. Miller, D. T., & Effron, D. A. (2010). Chapter three - psychological license: When it is needed and how it functions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 115-155. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2601(10)43003-8.
  28. Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 33-43. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.33
  29. Park, J., Kim, J., & Yoon. Y. (2016). The Effects of perceived popularity on prosocial behavior in elementary school students: The moderating effects of social connectivity and hierarchy in the peer relationships. Asian Journal of Education, 18(1), 25-49. DOI: 10.15753/aje.2017.03.18.1.25
  30. Rees, H. R., Fujita, K., Han, H. A., Sherman, J. W., & Sklar, A. Y. (2018). An examination of the processes by which construal level affects the implicit evaluation of goal relevant stimuli. Motivation Science, 4(3), 251-261. DOI: 10.1037/mot0000089
  31. Reed, A., II, Aquino, K., & Levy, E. (2007). Moral identity and judgments of charitable behaviors. Journal of Marketing, 71, 178-193. DOI: 10.1509/jmkg.71.1.178.
  32. Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., & Medin, D. L. (2009). Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science, 20(4), 523-528. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02326.x
  33. Stanley, M. L., Henne, P., Iyengar, V., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & De Brigard, F. (2017). I'm not the person I used to be: The self and autobiographical memories of immoral actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(6), 884-895. DOI: 10.1037/xge0000317
  34. Stanley, M. L., & De Brigard, F. (2019). Moral memories and the belief in the good self. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 387-391. DOI: 10.1177/0963721419847990
  35. Tasimi, A., & Young, L. (2016). Memories of good deeds past: The reinforcing power of prosocial behavior in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 159-166. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.001
  36. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403-421. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403