참고문헌
- Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219-235. DOI: 10.1177/1088868309341564
- Amaral, D. G., Price, J. L., Pitkanen, A., & Carmichael, S. T. (1992). Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction (pp. 1-66). New York: Wiley-Liss. DOI: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90106-i
- Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390-412. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
- Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. DOI: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
- Beligiannis, N., & Van Strien, J. W. (2019). Blurring attenuates the early posterior negativity in response to snake stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 146, 201-207. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.09.002
- Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436. DOI: 10.1163/156856897x00357
- Brewer, G. (2001, March 19). Snakes Top List of Americans' Fears. Gallup. https://news.gallup.com/poll/1891/snakes-top-list-americans-fears.aspx
- Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16(4), 372-389. DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.16.4.372
- Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Self‐reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: The role of disgust sensitivity. British Journal of Psychology, 85(4), 541-554. DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02540.x
- Eimer, M. (2000). Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 10(1-2), 145-158. DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00038-0
- Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., & Wik, G. (1997). Parental history, aversive exposure and the development of snake and spider phobia in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(1), 23-28. DOI: 10.1016/s0005-7967(96)00076-9
- Gayet, S., Stein, T., & Peelen, M. V. (2019). The danger of interpreting detection differences between image categories: A brief comment on "Mind the snake: Fear detection relies on low spatial frequencies" (Gomes, Soares, Silva, & Silva, 2018). Emotion, 19(5), 928-932. DOI: 10.1037/emo0000550
- Gayet, S., Van der Stigchel, S., & Paffen, C. L. E. (2014). Breaking continuous flash suppression: Competing for consciousness on the pre-semantic battlefield. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 460. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00460
- Gomes, N., Silva, S., Silva, C. F., & Soares, S. C. (2017). Beware the serpent: The advantage of ecologicallyrelevant stimuli in accessing visual awareness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(2), 227-234. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.004
- Gomes, N., Soares, S. C., Silva, S., & Silva, C. F. (2018). Mind the snake: Fear detection relies on low spatial frequencies. Emotion, 18(6), 886-895. DOI: 10.1037/emo0000391
- Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56(4), 544-549.
- Grassini, S., Holm, S. K., Railo, H., & Koivisto, M. (2016). Who is afraid of the invisible snake? Subjective visual awareness modulates posterior brain activity for evolutionarily threatening stimuli. Biological Psychology, 121(Pt A), 53-61. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.007
- Gray, K. L. H., Adams, W. J., Hedger, N., Newton, K. E., & Garner, M. (2013). Faces and awareness: Lowlevel, not emotional factors determine perceptual dominance. Emotion, 13(3), 537-544. DOI: 10.1037/a0031403
- Hayakawa, S., Kawai, N., & Masataka, N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Scientific Reports, 1, 80. DOI: 10.1038/srep00080
- Hedger, N., Adams, W. J., & Garner, M. (2015). Autonomic arousal and attentional orienting to visual threat are predicted by awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 798-806. DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000051
- Hedger, N., Gray, K. L. H., Garner, M., & Adams, W. J. (2016). Are visual threats prioritized without awareness? A critical review and meta-analysis involving 3 behavioral paradigms and 2696 observers. Psychological Bulletin, 142(9), 934-968. DOI: 10.1037/bul0000054
- He, H., Kubo, K., & Kawai, N. (2014). Spiders do not evoke greater early posterior negativity in the eventrelated potential as snakes. NeuroReport, 25(13), 1049-1053. DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000227
- Hendry, S. H., & Reid, R. C. (2000). The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 127-153. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.127
- Hendry, S. H., & Yoshioka, T. (1994). A neurochemically distinct third channel in the macaque dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Science, 264(5158), 575-577. DOI: 10.1126/science.8160015
- Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51(1), 1-35. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012
- Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434-446. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
- Jiang, Y., Costello, P., & He, S. (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: Advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychological Science, 18(4), 349-355. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x
- Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1-2), 1-19. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6
- Ju, E. J. (2014). Pre-service elementary teacher’s specific animal phobia and its relationship to biology teaching self-efficacy. Biology Education, 42(3), 237-248. DOI: 10.15717/bioedu.2014.42.3.237
- Keil, A., & Ihssen, N. (2004). Identification facilitation for emotionally arousing verbs during the attentional blink. Emotion, 4(1), 23-35. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.23
- Kim, S.-Y., Jung, J.-B., & Nam, K.-C. N. (2020). Research on the association between emotional perception bias and deteriorated visuospatial attention allocation ability in increasing the level of social phobia. Science of Emotion & Sensibility, 23(2), 35-50. DOI: 10.14695/KJSOS.2020.23.2.35
- Lamme, V. A., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571-579. DOI: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01657-x
- Langton, S. R. H., Law, A. S., Burton, A. M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Attention capture by faces. Cognition, 107(1), 330-342. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.012
- Lenth, R. (2020). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
- LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez- Edgar, K. (2017). Developmental differences in infants’ attention to social and nonsocial threats. Infancy, 22(3), 403-415. DOI: 10.1111/infa.12167
- Lobue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19(3), 284-289. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x
- Maior, R. S., Hori, E., Tomaz, C., Ono, T., & Nishijo, H. (2010). The monkey pulvinar neurons differentially respond to emotional expressions of human faces. Behavioural Brain Research, 215(1), 129-135. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.009
- Miles, W. R. (1930). Ocular dominance in human adults. The Journal of General Psychology, 3(3), 412-430. DOI: 10.1080/00221309.1930.9918218
- Mineka, S., Keir, R., & Price, V. (1980). Fear of snakes in wild- and laboratory-reared rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Learning & Behavior, 8(4), 653-663. DOI: 10.3758/BF03197783
- Moore, P. (2014, March 28). Snakes, heights and public speaking are the top three fears in the Home of the Brave. YouGov. https://today.yougov.com/topics/lifestyle/articles-reports/2014/03/27/argh-snakes
- Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393(6684), 467-470. DOI: 10.1038/30976
- Nguyen, M. N., Matsumoto, J., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., Tomaz, C., Tran, A. H., Ono, T., & Nishijo, H. (2014). Neuronal responses to face-like and facial stimuli in the monkey superior colliculus. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 85. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00085
- Ohman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & Behavior, 92(1-2), 180-185. DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.057
- Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130 (3), 466-478. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
- Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483-522. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483
- Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent: Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(1), 5-9. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.01211
- Ohman, A., Soares, S. C., Juth, P., Lindström, B., & Esteves, F. (2012). Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 17-32. DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603
- Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437-442. DOI: 10.1163/156856897x00366
- Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a "low road" to "many roads" of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 773-782. DOI: 10.1038/nrn2920
- Pessoa, L., Japee, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2005). Visual awareness and the detection of fearful faces. Emotion, 5(2), 243-247. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.243
- Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48(2), 175-187. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025
- Polak, J., Radlova, S., Janovcova, M., Flegr, J., Landova, E., & Frynta, D. (2020). Scary and nasty beasts: Self-reported fear and disgust of common phobic animals. British Journal of Psychology, 111(2), 297-321. DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12409
- Rakison, D. H. (2018). Do 5-month-old infants possess an evolved detection mechanism for snakes, sharks, and rodents? Journal of Cognition and Development, 19(4), 456-476. DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1488717
- R Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
- Roth, H. L., Lora, A. N., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Effects of monocular viewing and eye dominance on spatial attention. Brain, 125(Pt 9), 2023-2035. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awf210
- Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805-819. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805
- Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31-51. DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)56002-9
- Sheth, B. R., & Pham, T. (2008). How emotional arousal and valence influcence access to awareness, Vision Research, 48(23-24), 2415-2424. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.013
- Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., & Ben-Shachar, M. S. (2020). afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex
- Soares, S. C., Lindström, B., Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass: Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PloS One, 9(12), e114724. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114724
- Sohn, I.-J., Yoon, H.-J., Shin, Y.-B., & Kim, J.-J. (2014). Behavioral characteristics of face recognition for self and others in patients with social phobia. Anxiety and Mood, 10(1), 37-43.
- Stein, T., Seymour, K., Hebart, M. N., & Sterzer, P. (2014). Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies. Psychological Science, 25(2), 566-574. DOI: 10.1177/0956797613512509
- Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(10), 697-709. DOI: 10.1038/nrn2889
- Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13(6), 657-665. DOI: 10.1080/13506280500410949
- Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., Marcus, D. J., Westerlund, A., Casey, B. J., & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242-249. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
- Troiani, V., & Schultz, R. T. (2013). Amygdala, pulvinar, and inferior parietal cortex contribute to early processing of faces without awareness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 241. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00241
- Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096-1101. DOI: 10.1038/nn1500
- Tsuchiya, N., Moradi, F., Felsen, C., Yamazaki, M., & Adolphs, R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12(10), 1224-1225. DOI: 10.1038/nn.2380
- Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Nguyen, M., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., Tomaz, C., Tran, A. H., Ono, T., & Nishijo, H. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(47), 19000-19005. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1312648110
- Van Strien, J. W., Eijlers, R., Franken, I. H. A., & Huijding, J. (2014). Snake pictures draw more early attention than spider pictures in non-phobic women: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 96, 150-157. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.014
- Van Strien, J. W., Franken, I. H. A., & Huijding, J. (2014). Testing the snake-detection hypothesis: Larger early posterior negativity in humans to pictures of snakes than to pictures of other reptiles, spiders and slugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 691. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00691
- Van Strien, J. W., & Isbell, L. A. (2017). Snake scales, partial exposure, and the Snake Detection Theory: A human event-related potentials study. Scientific Reports, 7, 46331. DOI: 10.1038/srep46331
- Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Goffaux, V., & Kemner, C. (2009). Is the early modulation of brain activity by fearful facial expressions primarily mediated by coarse low spatial frequency information? Journal of Vision, 9(5), 12.1-13. DOI: 10.1167/9.5.12
- Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411-418. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-01-00411.1998
- Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiset, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., & Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 671-684. DOI: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.671
- Winston, J. S., Vuilleumier, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Effects of low-spatial frequency components of fearful faces on fusiform cortex activity. Current Biology, 13(20), 1824-1829. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2003.09.038
- Yang, E., Zald, D. H., & Blake, R. (2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion, 7(4), 882-886. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.882