DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Normalized gestural overlap measures and spatial properties of lingual movements in Korean non-assimilating contexts

  • Son, Minjung (Department of English Language and Literature, Hannam University)
  • Received : 2019.08.16
  • Accepted : 2019.09.17
  • Published : 2019.09.30

Abstract

The current electromagnetic articulography study analyzes several articulatory measures and examines whether, and if so, how they are interconnected, with a focus on cluster types and an additional consideration of speech rates and morphosyntactic contexts. Using articulatory data on non-assimilating contexts from three Seoul-Korean speakers, we examine how speaker-dependent gestural overlap between C1 and C2 in a low vowel context (/a/-to-/a/) and their resulting intergestural coordination are realized. Examining three C1C2 sequences (/k(#)t/, /k(#)p/, and /p(#)t/), we found that three normalized gestural overlap measures (movement onset lag, constriction onset lag, and constriction plateau lag) were correlated with one another for all speakers. Limiting the scope of analysis to C1 velar stop (/k(#)t/ and /k(#)p/), the results are recapitulated as follows. First, for two speakers (K1 and K3), i) longer normalized constriction plateau lags (i.e., less gestural overlap) were observed in the pre-/t/ context, compared to the pre-/p/ (/k(#)t/>/k(#)p/), ii) the tongue dorsum at the constriction offset of C1 in the pre-/t/ contexts was more anterior, and iii) these two variables are correlated. Second, the three speakers consistently showed greater horizontal distance between the vertical tongue dorsum and the vertical tongue tip position in /k(#)t/ sequences when it was measured at the time of constriction onset of C2 (/k(#)t/>/k(#)p/): the tongue tip completed its constriction onset by extending further forward in the pre-/t/ contexts than the uncontrolled tongue tip articulator in the pre-/p/ contexts (/k(#)t/>/k(#)p/). Finally, most speakers demonstrated less variability in the horizontal distance of the lingual-lingual sequences, which were taken as the active articulators (/k(#)t/=/k(#)p/ for K1; /k(#)t/

Keywords

References

  1. Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
  2. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. Articulatory phonology. Unpublished manuscript.
  3. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. M. (1986). Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 219-252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700000658
  4. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1988). Some notes on syllable structure in articulatory phonology. Phonetica, 45(2-4), 140-155. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261823
  5. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology, 6(2), 201-251. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700001019
  6. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1990). Gestural specification using dynamically-defined articulatory structures. Journal of Phonetics, 18, 299-320. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30376-6
  7. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1991). Tiers in articulatory phonology, with some implications for casual speech. In J. Kingston, & M. E. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology I: Between the grammar and the physics of speech (pp. 341-376).
  8. Cambridge, UK.: Cambridge University Press. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica, 49, 155-180. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261913
  9. Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1995). Gestural syllable position effects in American English. In F. Bell-Berti, & R. J. Lawrence (Eds.), Producing speech: Contemporary issues. For Katherine Safford Harris (pp. 19-33). New York, NY: AIP Press.
  10. Brunner, J., Fuchs, S., & Perrier, P. (2009). On the relationship between palate shape and articulatory behavior. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), 3936-3949. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3125313
  11. Byrd, D. (1996). Influences on articulatory timing in consonant sequences. Journal of Phonetics, 24(2), 209-244. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1996.0012
  12. Chitoran, I., Goldstein, L., & Byrd, D. (2002). Gestural overlap and recoverability: Articulatory evidence from Georgian. In C. Gussenhoven, & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VII (pp. 419-448). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  13. Cho, T. (2001). Effects of morpheme boundaries on intergestural timing: Evidence from Korean. Phonetica, 58(3), 129-162. https://doi.org/10.1159/000056196
  14. Gay, T. (1977). Articulatory movements in VCV sequences. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, 49, 121-147.
  15. Jun, J. (1995). Perceptual and articulatory factors in place assimilation: An optimality theoretic approach (Ph.D. dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles.
  16. Kelso, J. A. S., Saltzman, E., L., & Tuller, B. (1986). The dynamical perspective on speech production: Data and theory. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 29-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30608-4
  17. Kirchner, R. M. (1998). An effort-based approach to consonant lenition (Ph.D. dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles.
  18. Kochetov, A., Pouplier, M., & Son, M. (2007, August). Cross-language differences in overlap and assimilation patterns in Korean and Russian. Proceedings of the 16th International Congress International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 1361-1364). Saarbrucken.
  19. Kuhnert, B., Hoole, P., & Mooshammer, C. (2006, December). Gestural overlap and C-center in selected French consonant clusters. Proceedings of the 7th International Seminar on Speech Production (pp. 327-334). Ubatuba.
  20. Lin, S., Beddor, P. S., & Coetzee, A. W. (2014). Gestural reduction, lexical frequency, and sound change: A study of post-vocalic /l/. Laboratory Phonology, 5(1), 9-36. https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0002
  21. Marin, S., & Pouplier, M. (2014). Articulatory synergies in the temporal organization of liquid clusters in Romanian. Journal of Phonetics, 42, 24-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.11.001
  22. Mooshammer, C., & Hoole, P. (1993). Articulation and coarticulation in velar consonants. Forschungsberichte des Instituts fur Phonetik und Sprachliche Kommunikation der Universitat Munchen FIPKM, 31, 249-262.
  23. Mooshammer, C., Hoole, P., & Kuhnert, B. (1995). On loops. Journal of Phonetics, 23(1-2), 3-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(95)80029-8
  24. Nam, H. (2007). A competitive, coupled oscillator model of moraic structure: Split-gesture dynamics focusing on positional asymmetry. In J. Cole, & J. I. Hualde (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology IX (pp. 483-506). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  25. Nam, H., Goldstein, L. M., Giulivi, S., Levitt, A. G., & Whalen, D. H. (2013). Computational simulation of CV combination preferences in babbling. Journal of Phonetics, 41(2), 63-77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2012.11.002
  26. Nam, H., Goldstein, L., & Saltzman, E. (2009). Self-organization of syllable structure: A coupled oscillator model. In F. Pellegrino, E. Marisco, I. Chitoran, & C. Coupe (Eds.), Approaches to phonological complexity (pp. 299-328). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  27. Oliveira, L., & Marin, S. (2005). Patterns of velum coordination in Brazilian Portuguese. Phonetics and Phonology in Iberia (PaPi). Barcelona, Spain.
  28. Perkell, J., Cohen, M. H., Svirsky, M. A., Matthies, M. L., Garabieta, I., & Jackson, M. T.. (1992). Electromagnetic midsagittal articulometer (EMMA) systems for transducing speech articulatory movements. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92(6), 3078-3096. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.404204
  29. Pouplier, M., Marin, S., Hoole, P., & Kochetov, A. (2017). Speech rate effects in Russian onset clusters are modulated by frequency, but not auditory cue robustness. Journal of Phonetics, 64, 108-126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.01.006
  30. R Development Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org
  31. Saltzman, E. L., & Munhall, K. G. (1989). A dynamical patterning to gestural patterning in speech production. Ecological Psychology, 1(4), 333-382. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0104_2
  32. Saltzman, E., & Kelso, J. A. (1987). Skilled actions: A task-dynamic approach. Psychological Review, 94(1), 84-106. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.84
  33. Son, M. (2008). Gestural overlap as a function of assimilation contrast. Korean Journal of Linguistics, 33(4), 665-691. https://doi.org/10.18855/lisoko.2008.33.4.005
  34. Son, M. (2011). A language-specific physiological motor constraint in Korean non-assimilating consonant sequences. Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 3(3), 27-33.
  35. Son, M. (2015). Articulatory properties of the allophonic variant [ɾ] in Korean /l/-flapping: Gestural reduction and the role of gestural overlap. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, 21(3), 427-456.
  36. Son, M., Kochetov, A., & Pouplier, M. (2007). The role of gestural overlap in perceptual place assimilation in Korean. In J. Cole, & J. Hualde (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology IX (pp. 507-534). New York : Mouton de Gruyter.
  37. Tiede, M. (2005). MVIEW: Software for visualization and analysis of concurrently recorded movement data. New Haven, CT: Haskins Laboratories.
  38. Vihman, M. M., & Greenlee, M. (1987). Individual differences in phonological development: Ages one and three years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30(4), 503-521. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3004.503
  39. Weirich, M. (2010). Articulatory and acoustic inter-speaker variability in the production of German vowels. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 52, 19-42. https://doi.org/10.21248/zaspil.52.2010.381