DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Characteristics of Visuo-Spatial Information Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Kwon, Mee-Kyoung (Division of General Studies, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology) ;
  • Chung, Hee-Jung (Department of Pediatrics, National Health Insurance Service Ilsan Hospital) ;
  • Song, Hyunjoo (Department of Psychotherapy, Seoul Women's University)
  • 투고 : 2018.04.04
  • 심사 : 2018.06.18
  • 발행 : 2018.06.30

초록

Although atypical sensory processing is a core feature of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), there is considerable heterogeneity among ASD individuals in the modality and symptoms of atypical sensory processing. The present study examined visual processing of children with ASD, focusing on the complexity and orientation of visual information. Age- and -IQ-matched Korean children (14 ASD and 14 typically-developing (TD) children) received an orientation discrimination task involving static spatial gratings varied in complexity (simple versus complex) and orientation (horizontal versus vertical). The results revealed that ASD children had difficulty perceiving complex information regardless of orientation, whereas TD children had more difficulty with vertical gratings than horizontal gratings. Thus, group-level differences between ASD and TD children appeared greater when gratings were presented horizontally. Unlike ASD adult literature, however, ASD children did not show superior performance on simple gratings. Our findings on typical and atypical processing of ASD children have implications for both understanding the characteristics of ASD children and developing diagnostic tools for ASD.

키워드

참고문헌

  1. Allison, C., Williams, J., Scott, F., Stott, C., Bolton, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Brayne, C. (2007). The Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test (CAST):Test-retest reliability in a high scoring sample. Autism, 11(2), 173-185. DOI:10.1177/1362361307075710
  2. Armstrong, V., Maurer, D., & Lewis, T. L. (2009). Sensitivity to first- and second-order motion and form in children and adults. Vision Research, 49(23), 2774-2781. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.016
  3. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (fifth ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  4. Bertone, A., & Faubert, J. (2006). Demonstrations of decreased sensitivity to complex motion information not enough to propose an autism-specific neural etiology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 55-64. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-005-0042-5
  5. Bertone, A., Hanck, J., Cornish, K. M., & Faubert, J. (2008). Development of static and dynamic perception for luminance-defined and texture-defined information. Neuroreport, 19(2), 225-228. DOI:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f48401
  6. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: a "complex" issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 218-225. DOI: 10.1162/089892903321208150
  7. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(10), 2430-2441. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awh561
  8. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436. DOI: 10.1163/156856897X00357
  9. Brosseau-Lachaine, O., Gagnon, I., Forget, R., & Faubert, J. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury induces prolonged visual processing deficits in children. Brain Injury, 22(9), 657-668. DOI: 10.1080/02699050802203353
  10. Chlebowski, C., Green, J. A., Barton, M. L., & Fein, D. (2010). Using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale to Diagnose Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(7), 787-799. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-009-0926-x
  11. Dumoulin, S. O., Baker, C. L., Hess, R. F., & Evans, A. C. (2003). Cortical specialization for processing first- and second-order motion. Cerebral Cortex, 13(12), 1375-1385. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhg085
  12. Ellemberg, D., Lavoie, K., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., Lepore, F., & Guillemot, J. P. (2003). Longer VEP latencies and slower reaction times to the onset of second-order motion than to the onset of first-order motion. Vision Research, 43(6), 651-658. DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00006-3
  13. Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Meghji, K. S., Maurer, D., Guillemot, J. P., & Lepore, F. (2003). Comparison of sensitivity to first- and second-order local motion in 5-year-olds and adults. Spatial Vision, 16(5), 419-428. DOI: 10.1163/156856803322552748
  14. Farzin, F., Whitney, D., Hagerman, R. J., & Rivera, S. M. (2008). Contrast detection in infants with fragile X syndrome. Vision Research, 48(13), 1471-1478. DOI:10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.019
  15. Gottsdanker, R., & Tietz, J. D. (1992). Reaction time for horizontal versus vertical line-length discrimination. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30(1), 74-76. DOI:10.3758/bf03330401
  16. Keita, L., Guy, J., Berthiaume, C., Mottron, L., & Bertone, A. (2014). An early origin for detailed perception in Autism Spectrum Disorder: biased sensitivity for high-spatial frequency information. Scientific Reports, 4, 5475. DOI:10.1038/srep05475
  17. Koh, H. C., Milne, E., & Dobkins, K. (2010). Spatial contrast sensitivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 978-987. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-010-0953-7
  18. Kwak, K. J., Park, H. W., & Kim, C. T. (2001). Korean version Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition. Special Education Publishing Co, Seoul, Korea.
  19. Kwak, K. J., Oh, S. W., & Kim, C. T. (2011). Korean version Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV. Hakjisa, Seoul, Korea.
  20. Lachapelle, J., Ouimet, C., Bach, M., Ptito, A., & McKerral, M. (2004). Texture segregation in traumatic brain injury--a VEP study. Vision Research, 44(24), 2835-2842. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.06.007
  21. Marco, E. J., Hinkley, L. B., Hill, S. S., & Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatric Research, 69(5 Pt 2), 48R-54R. DOI:10.1203/PDR.0b013e3182130c54
  22. Meilleur, A. A., Berthiaume, C., Bertone, A., & Mottron, L. (2014). Autism-specific covariation in perceptual performances: "g" or "p" factor? Plos One, 9(8), e103781. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103781
  23. Parrish, E. E., Giaschi, D. E., Boden, C., & Dougherty, R. (2005). The maturation of form and motion perception in school age children. Vision Research, 45(7), 827-837. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.005
  24. Rivest, J. B., Jemel, B., Bertone, A., McKerral, M., & Mottron, L. (2013). Luminance- and texture-defined information processing in school-aged children with autism. Plos One, 8(10), e78978. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078978
  25. Sanchez-Marin, F. J., & Padilla-Medina, J. A. (2008). A psychophysical test of the visual pathway of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1270-1277. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-007-0507-9
  26. Swets, J. A. (1988). Measuring the accuracy of diagnostic systems. Science, 240(4857), 1285-1293. DOI: 10.1126/science.3287615
  27. Sysoeva, O. V., Davletshina, M. A., Orekhova, E. V., Galuta, I. A., & Stroganova, T. A. (2015). Reduced Oblique Effect in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 512. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00512
  28. Vandenbroucke, M. W., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A., & Kemner, C. (2008). A neural substrate for atypical low-level visual processing in autism spectrum disorder. Brain, 131(4), 1013-1024. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awm321
  29. Waterhouse, L., London, E., & Gillberg, C. (2016). ASD Validity. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(4), 302-329. DOI: 10.1007/s40489-016-0085-x
  30. Wilkes, B. J., Carson, T. B., Patel, K. P., Lewis, M. H., & White, K. D. (2015). Oculomotor performance in children with high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 38, 338-344. DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.12.02