DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Inaction Inertia Effect - Moderation Effect of Habitual Purchase Behavior and Maximization Behavior -

무행동관성효과 - 습관적 구매성향과 극대화성향의 조절효과를 중심으로 -

  • Received : 2018.11.09
  • Accepted : 2018.12.17
  • Published : 2018.12.31

Abstract

Inaction inertia effect refers to the phenomenon wherein people who missed attractive opportunities in the past do not take any action when they are given the opportunity to receive discounts (sale). The main objective of this study was to find out how such inaction inertia effect influenced the expected regret from the action, expected regret from inaction, and purchase intention depending on the two types of consumption purposes-for pleasure and for practical use. As the personal disposition of each consumer differs from one another, it was expected that habitual purchase behavior and maximization behavior would influence the inaction inertia. An experiment was thus conducted by using a scenario to satisfy the objective of this study. Results showed that in the case of consumers whose consumption purpose was for pleasure, those who habitually purchased the same brand showed a higher level of expected regret from the action and expected regret from inaction. In the case of maximization behavior, no differences were found in the expected regret from the action between practical consumption purpose and pleasurable consumption purpose. On the other hand, differences were found in the level of expected regret from inaction, which is the case wherein consumers who missed their first opportunity to receive discounts (sale) did not make any purchase on their second opportunity for discounts (sale). Thus, inaction inertia was shown in accordance with habitual purchase behavior in the case of consumption purpose for pleasure, but it was not clearly shown in the case of consumption for practical use. This is because there is a relatively low level of justification on consumption, in the case of consumption for practical use. On the other hand, it was found that consumers with maximization behavior felt a stronger sense of regret in the case of their expected regret for inaction in consumption for practical use than in consumption for pleasure. Also, with regard to purchase intention, it turned out that only consumers with pleasurable consumption purpose had purchase intentions. Through these study results, it would be necessary to consider the consumption purpose behind the purchase of products or services, with regard to inaction inertia, depending on personal dispositions. In accordance with these study results, several theoretical and practical implications were discussed.

무행동 관성효과는 과거의 매력적인 기회를 놓친 사람들이 이후 할인(세일)기회가 주어져도 그것을 얻기 위한 행동을 하지 않는 현상을 말한다. 본 연구의 기본 목적은 이런 무행동 관성효과가 소비목표에 따라 쾌락적인 소비목표를 가지는 경우와 실용적인 소비목표를 가질 때를 구분하여 예상된 행동후회와 예상된 무행동 후회 및 구매의도에 어떠한 영향을 미치는지 규명하고자 하였다. 이는 소비자의 개인적 성향이 다르게 나타나기 때문에 습관적 구매성향과 극대화 성향이 무행동 관성에 영향을 주는 요소가 될 것임을 예상하였다. 연구목적 달성을 위하여 시나리오를 사용하여 실험을 진행하였으며 연구결과 쾌락적 소비목표를 가진 소비자의 경우 습관적으로 동일한 브랜드를 구매하는 소비자에서 예상된 행동후회와 예상된 무행동 후회 수준이 더 높았다. 그리고 극대화 성향의 경우 예상된 행동후회에 대해서는 실용적 소비목표와 쾌락적 소비목표에 따른 차이가 발견되지 않았다. 반면 첫 번째 할인(세일)기회를 놓친 후 두 번째 할인(세일)기회에 구매를 하지 않는 후회수준인 예상된 무행동 후회에서는 차이가 나타났다. 따라서 소비목표가 쾌락적인 경우에는 습관적 구매성향에 따라 무행동 관성행동이 나타났으나 실용적 소비목표에서는 무행동 관성효과가 강하게 나타나지 않았다. 이는 실용적 소비목표의 경우 소비에 대한 정당화가 상대적으로 적기 때문이라고 설명된다. 반면 극대화 성향의 소비자의 경우 쾌락적 소비목표에서보다 실용적 소비목표에서 예상된 무행동 후회에 더 강한 후회를 한다는 것을 알 수 있었다. 또한 구매의도와 관련해서는 쾌락적 소비목표를 가진 소비자들에게서만 구매의도가 있는 것으로 나타났다. 결국 연구결과를 통해 무행동 관성행동이 나타나는 것은 제품이나 서비스가 어떠한 소비목표를 가지고 소비되는지 개인적인 성향에 따라 고려하는 것이 필요하다. 이러한 결과에 따른 몇 가지 이론적 및 실무적 시사점을 논의하였다.

Keywords

References

  1. 구현진.손영우.임혜빈(2016), "소비자의 기질적 욕심이 무행동 관성에 미치는 영향 : 예상되는 무행동 후회와 극대화 성향의 조절된 매개효과", 소비자학연구, 27(5), 305-321
  2. 김승화.안희경(2017), "무행동관성 : 지각된 손실과 소비동기의 상호작용", 광고학연구, 29(1), 107-125
  3. 김형식.배일현(2014), "소비자의 점포이용 목적이 점포만족에 미치는 영향 : 극대화 성향과 조절모드의 조절적 역할을 중심으로", 유통경영학회지, 17(3), 125-136.
  4. 박지숙.양윤(2007), "의사결정 상황에서 행동과 비행동에 따라 경험하는 후회", 한국심리학회지: 소비자.광고, 8(1), 1-33.
  5. 박종일.손현국.손영우(2012), "무행동 관성(inaction inertia)이 소비자의 구매의도에 미치는 영향", 한국심리학회지: 소비자.광고, 13(1), 101-118.
  6. 양윤.전규민(2009), "성향 및 상황 조절초점이 행동/무행동에 대한 소비자 후회에 미치는 영향", 한국심리학회지: 소비자.광고, 10(3), 513-533.
  7. 이성수(2014), "무행동 관성효과 : 조절초점의 조절효과", 광고학 연구, 25(6), 27-52.
  8. 이성수.우석봉.김정식(2012), "무행동 관성효과 : 소비자의 성별과 낙관주의-비낙관주의의 조절", 경영컨설팅연구, 12(4), 101-121.
  9. 이은경.전중옥(2016), "습관적 구매유형에 따른 소비자의 양면적 반응", 마케팅 연구, 31(8), 47-74.
  10. 이은경.전중옥(2017), "습관적 구매 유형에 따른 소비자의 양면적 행동: 습관변화 시도에 따른 반응을 중심으로", 서비스마케팅저널, 10(2), 5-22.
  11. 이성수(2014), "무행동 관성 효과: 조절초점의 조절효과", 광고학연구, 25(6), 27-52.
  12. 임재문(2010), "제품편익과 소비자-브랜드 관계 및 브랜드 애착, 구전간의 구조적 관계에 관란 연구", 경영정보연구, 29(1) 117-144.
  13. 서문식.하승범(2014), "충동구매 매커니즘에 관한 연구", 소비자학연구, 25(5), 29-62.
  14. 한광석(2005), "브랜드 아이덴티티의 가치제안요소가 성별에 따라 태도와 구매의도에 미치는 영향", 대한경영정보연구, 17, 1-14.,
  15. 허정록.조정화(207), "가격할인제시 방식에 따를 브랜드이미지와 구매의도에 미치는 영향", 경영과정보연구, 36(1), 21-40.
  16. Aspinwall, L. G.(1998), "Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation", Motivation and emotion, 22(1), 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023080224401
  17. Assael, H.(1998), "Customer behavior and marketing action". southwestern college publishing, cincinnati, OH.
  18. Anderson, C. J.(2003), "The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion", Psychological bulletin, 129(1), 139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.139
  19. Bell, D. E.(1982), "Regret in decision making under uncertainty", Operations research, 30(5), 961-981. https://doi.org/10.1287/opre.30.5.961
  20. Childers, T. L., Carr, C. L., Peck, J., and Carson, S.(2001), "Hedonic and utilitarian motivations for online retail shopping behavior", Journal of retailing, 77(4), 511-535. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00056-2
  21. Dar-Nimrod, I., Rawn, C. D., Lehman, D. R., and Schwartz, B.(2009), "The maximization paradox: The costs of seeking alternatives", Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5-6), 631-635. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.007
  22. Dhar, R., and Wertenbroch, K.(2000), "Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods", Journal of marketing research, 37(1), 60-71. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.37.1.60.18718
  23. Ehrenberg, A., and Goodhardt, G.(2002), "Double jeopardy revisited, again", Marketing Research, 14(1), 40.
  24. Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., and Schwartz, B.(2006), "Doing better but feeling worse: Looking for the"best" job undermines satisfaction", Psychological Science, 17(2), 143-150. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01677.x
  25. Jamieson, L. F., and Bass, F. M.(1989), "Adjusting stated intention measures to predict trial purchase of new products: A comparison of models and methods", Journal of Marketing Research, 336-345.
  26. Kahneman, D., and Miller, D. T.(1986), "Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives", Psychological review, 93(2), 136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.136
  27. Keinan, A., and Kivetz, R.(2008), "Remedying hyperopia: The effects of self-control regret on consumer behavior", Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 676-689. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.45.6.676
  28. Leclerc, F., Schmitt, B. H., and Dube, L.(1994), "Foreign branding and its effects on product perceptions and attitudes", Journal of marketing Research, 263-270
  29. Liu, T. C., Cheng, T., and Ni, F. Y.(2011), "How consumers respond to the behavior of missing a free gift promotion: Inaction inertia effect on products offered as free gifts", The Journal of social psychology, 151(3), 361-381. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2010.483698
  30. Loomes, G., and Sugden, R.(1982), "Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty", The economic Journal, 92(368), 805-824. https://doi.org/10.2307/2232669
  31. Martin, N.(2008), Habit: The 95% of behavior marketers ignore upper saddle river, NJ: FT Press.
  32. Murray, K. B., and Haubl, G.(2007), "Explaining cognitive lock-in: The role of skill-based habits of use in consumer choice", Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 77-88. https://doi.org/10.1086/513048
  33. Neal, D. T., Wood, W., and Quinn, J. M. (2006), "Habits-A repeat performance. Current Directions", Psychological Science, 15(4), 198-202.
  34. Okada, Erica M.(2005), "Justification Effects on ConsumerChoice of Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods," Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 43-53. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.42.1.43.56889
  35. Patrick, V. M., Lancellotti, M. and Hagtvedt, H.(2009), "Getting a second chance: the role of imagery in the influence of inaction regret on behavioral intent", Journal of the academy of marketing science, 37(2), 181-190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-008-0119-8
  36. Roese, N. J., and Hur, T.(1997), "Affective determinants of counterfactual thinking", Social Cognition, 15(4), 274-290. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1997.15.4.274
  37. Roese, N. J., and Olson, J. M.(1997), "Counterfactual thinking: The intersection of affect and function", Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 29, 1-59
  38. Sanna, L. J., and Turley, K. J.(1996), "Antecedents to spontaneous counterfactual thinking: Effects of expectancy violation and outcome valence", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(9), 906-919. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296229005
  39. Sevdalis, N., Harvey, N., and Yip, M. (2006), "Regret triggers inaction inertia-but which regret and how?", British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(4), 839-853. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X84790
  40. Shah, D., Kumar, V., and Kim, K. H.(2014), "Managing customer profits: The power of habits", Journal of Marketing Research, 51(6), 726-741. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.13.0423
  41. Shi, Y., and Zhang, B.(2016), "Recent advances in transition metal phosphide nanomaterials: synthesis and applications in hydrogen evolution reaction", Chemical Society Reviews, 45(6), 1529-1541. https://doi.org/10.1039/C5CS00434A
  42. Teng, L., Laroche, M., and Zhu, H.(2007), "The effects of multiple-ads and multiplebrands on consumer attitude and purchase behavior", Journal of Consumer Marketing, 24(1), 27-35. https://doi.org/10.1108/07363760710720966
  43. Tsiros, M.(2008), "Releasing the regret lock: Consumer response to new alternatives after a sale", Journal of Consumer Research, 35(6), 1039-1059. https://doi.org/10.1086/593698
  44. Tykocinski, O. E., and Pittman, T. S.(1998), "The consequences of doing nothing: Inaction inertia as avoidance of anticipated counterfactual regret", Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(3), 607. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.607
  45. Tykocinski, O. E., Pittman, T. S., and Tuttle, E. E.(1995), "Inaction inertia: Foregoing future benefits as a result of an initial failure to act", Journal of personality and social psychology, 68(5), 793. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.793
  46. Tykocinski, O. E., and Pittman, T. S.(2001), "Product aversion following a missed opportunity: price contrast or avoidance of anticipated regret?", Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23(3), 149-156. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324834BASP2303_1
  47. Van Putten, M., Zeelenberg, M., and Van Dijk, E.(2009), "Dealing with missed opportunities: Action vs. state orientation moderates inaction inertia", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 808-815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.011
  48. Van Putten, M., Zeelenberg, M., and Van Dijk, E.(2013), "How consumers deal with missed discounts: Transaction decoupling, action orientation and inaction inertia", Journal of Economic Psychology, 38, 104-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2012.09.008
  49. Verplanken, B., and Orbell, S.(2003), "Reflections on Past Behavior: A Self‐ Report Index of Habit Strength 1", Journal of applied social psychology, 33(6), 1313-1330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x
  50. Verplanken, B., and Wood, W.(2006), "Interventions to break and create consumer habits", Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25(1), 90-103. https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.25.1.90
  51. Voss, K. E., Spangenberg, E. R., and Grohmann, B.(2003), "Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian dimensions of consumer attitude", Journal of marketing research, 40(3), 310-320. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.40.3.310.19238
  52. Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., and Lehman, D. R.(2002), "Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice", Journal of personality and social psychology, 83(5), 1178. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1178
  53. Schwartz, Barry(2004), The Paradox of Choice, New York : Eceo.
  54. Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., and Lehman, D. R.(2002), "Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice", Journal of personality and social psychology, 83(5), 1178. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1178
  55. Sevdalis, N., Harvey, N., and Yip, M.(2006), "Regret triggers inaction inertia-but which regret and how?", British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(4), 839-853. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X84790
  56. Zeelenberg, M., Van Dijk, W. W., and Manstead, A. S.(1998), "Reconsidering the relation between regret and responsibility", Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 74(3), 254-272. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2780
  57. Zeelenberg, M., and Putten, M. V.(2005), "The dark side of discounts: An inaction inertia perspective on the post‐promotion dip", Psychology & Marketing, 22(8), 611-622. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20076

Cited by

  1. 항공여객이 인식하는 편익이 가격신뢰를 매개로 재구매 의도에 미치는 영향 : 인천-두바이 구간 국적항공사와 외국항공사 비교를 중심으로 vol.24, pp.3, 2018, https://doi.org/10.12673/jant.2020.24.3.173