DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Development of Spatial Geometry Cognition in 3-, 4-, and 5-Year-Old

3, 4, 5세 유아의 공간기하 인지 발달

  • Kim, Bokyung (Department of Child Development and Family Studies, College of Human Ecology, Seoul National University) ;
  • Yi, Soon Hyung (Department of Child Development and Family Studies, College of Human Ecology, Seoul National University)
  • 김보경 (서울대학교 생활과학대학 아동가족학과) ;
  • 이순형 (서울대학교 생활과학대학 아동가족학과)
  • Received : 2016.11.18
  • Accepted : 2016.12.27
  • Published : 2017.04.30

Abstract

This study composed spatial cognition tasks within the system of geometric area to study children's spatial cognition development systematically. It surveyed children's execution of direction, rotation, symmetry, conjugation, and part/whole cognition tasks. A spatial geometry cognition task set (consisting of total 27 sub-tasks) was presented to 60 children (20 each in groups of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old) in order to confirm how children's execution of spatial geometry cognition changed depending on children's age and sex as well as if the execution of the spatial geometry cognition showed a difference after each task area. As a result, the execution of the whole direction task and the part/whole task gradually increased between age 3 and age 5. The execution of the whole rotation task, whole symmetry task, and whole conjugation task rapidly increased between age 3 and age 4. Significant sexual difference did not appear in the execution of spatial geometry cognition tasks. The execution of the conjugation and part/whole task was high in each task area, and the execution of the direction, rotation, and symmetry task was relatively low. In addition, the difference of task execution appeared in the sub-tasks of direction, symmetry, and conjugation areas. This result suggests the theoretical discussion possibility of children's spatial geometry cognition development. In addition, the empirical results of this study can be applied to child education plans and activity compositions appropriate for child development.

Keywords

References

  1. Beilin, H. (1984). Cognitive theory and mathematical cognition: Geometry and space. In B. Gholson & T. L. Rosenthal (Eds.), Applications of cognitive-developmental theory (pp. 49-69). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
  2. Belmont, L., & Birch, H. G. (1963). Lateral dominance and right-left awareness in normal children. Child Development, 34(2), 257-270. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126726
  3. Bialystok, E. (1989). Children's mental rotations of abstract displays. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47(1), 47-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(89)90062-3
  4. Carpenter, T. P. (1979). Research in children's thinking and the design of mathematics instruction. In R. Lesh, D. Mierkiewicz, & M. Kantrowski (Eds.), Applied mathematical problem solving (pp. 207-228). Columbus, OH: ERIC/SMEAC.
  5. Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
  6. Case, R., & Griffin, S. (1990). Child cognitive development: The role of central conceptual structures in the development of scientific and social thought. In C. A. Hauert (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Cognitive, perceptuo-motor, and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 193-230). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
  7. Choi, Y., & Lee, J. W. (2006). Motion geometry concepts in young children. The Journal of Korea Open Association for Early Childhood Education, 11(1), 197-215.
  8. Clark, E. V. (1980). Here’s the “top”: Nonlinguistic strategies in the acquisition of orientational terms. Child Development, 51(2), 329-338. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129265
  9. Clements, D. H. (1999). Your child’s geometric world. Scholastic Parent and Child, 7(2), 48-54.
  10. Clements, D. H. (2004). Geometric and spatial thinking in early childhood education. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A. M. Dibiases (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standard for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 267-297). Mhawah, VJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  11. Del Grande, J. (1990). Spatial sense. The Arithmetic Teacher, 37(6), 14-20.
  12. Dodwell, P. C. (1963). Children’s understanding of spatial concepts. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17(1), 141-161. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083273
  13. Fisher, C. B., & Braine, L. G. (1981). Children’s left-right concepts: Generalization across figure and location. Child Development, 52(2), 451-456. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129161
  14. Frick, A., Hansen, M. A., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). Development of mental rotation in 3-to 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 28(4), 386-399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.06.002
  15. Frick, A., & Mohring, W. (2013). Mental object rotation and motor development in 8-and 10-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 708-720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.001
  16. Fuson, K. C. (1978). An analysis of research needs in projective, affine and similarity geometries, including an evaluation of Piaget's results in those areas. In R. Lesh & D. Mierkiewicz (Eds.), Recent research concerning the development of spatial and geometric concepts (pp. 243-260). Columbus, OH: ERIC/SMEAC.
  17. Gentner, D., Ozyurek, A., Gurcanli, O., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input. Cognition, 127(3), 318-330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.003
  18. Hahn, N., Jansen, P., & Hell, M. (2010). Preschoolers’ mental rotation of letters: Sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 261-267. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2010.485248
  19. Halford, G. S. (2004). Information-processing models of cognitive development. In U. Goswami (Ed.). Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 555-574). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
  20. Harris, J., Newcombe, N. S., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013). A new twist on studying the development of dynamic spatial transformations: Mental paper folding in young children. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7(1), 49-55. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12007
  21. Harvey, C. B., Manshu, Z., Biao, K. C., & Jue, Z. F. (1986). Spatial conceptions in Chinese and Canadian children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147(4), 457-464. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1986.9914522
  22. Hawes, Z., LeFevre, J. A., Xu, C., & Bruce, C. D. (2015). Mental rotation with tangible three-dimensional objects: A new measure sensitive to developmental differences in 4-to 8-year-old children. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9(1), 10-18. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12051
  23. Hirnstein, M., Ocklenburg, S., Schneider, D., & Hausmann, M. (2009). Sex differences in left-right confusion depend on hemispheric asymmetry. Cortex, 45(7), 891-899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.009
  24. Jansen, P., Schmelter, A., Quaiser-Pohl, C., Neuburger, S., & Heil, M. (2013). Mental rotation performance in primary school age children: Are there gender differences in chronometric test? Cognitive Development, 28(1), 51-62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.08.005
  25. Jedrysek, E., Klapper, Z., Pope, L., & Wortis, J. (1972). Psychoeducational evaluation of the preschool child. New York, NY: Grune & Stratton.
  26. Kail, R. (1988). Developmental functions for speeds of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45(3), 339-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(88)90036-7
  27. Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109(3), 490-501. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.109.3.490
  28. Kail, R., & Park, Y. (1992). Global developmental change in processing time. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(4), 525-541.
  29. Kennedy, L. M., Tipps, S., & Johnson, A. (2004). Guiding children's learning of mathematics. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
  30. Kim, H. S. (2005). The effect of children's body activities on young children's spatial concept (Unpublished master's thesis). Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Korea.
  31. Kim, K. H. (1981). Development of conceptions of right and left in Korean children. Korean Journal of Early Childhood Education, 3, 5-13.
  32. Kruger, M., Kaiser, M., Mahler, K., Bartels, W., & Krist, H. (2014). Analogue mental transformations in 3-year-olds: Introducing a new mental rotation paradigm suitable for young children. Infant and Child Development, 23(2), 123-138. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1815
  33. Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). A modular geometric mechanism for reorientation in children. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 152-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.04.002
  34. Levine, S. C., Foley, A., Lourenco, S., Ehrlich, S., & Ratliff, K. (2016). Sex differences in spatial cognition: Advancing the conversation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Review: Cognitive Science, 7(2), 127-155. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1380
  35. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479-1498. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130467
  36. Maki, R. H., Grandy, C. A., & Hauge, G. (1979). Why is telling right from left more difficult than telling above from below? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5(1), 52-67. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.5.1.52
  37. Martin, J. L. (1976). An analysis of some of Piaget’s topological tasks from a mathematical point of view. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 7(1), 8-24. https://doi.org/10.2307/748762
  38. McGee, M. G. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychological Bulletin, 86(5), 889-918. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.86.5.889
  39. Nah, K. O. (2006). An analysis of standards of geometry education for young children in US and Korea. Journal of Future Early Childhood Education, 13(3), 163-184.
  40. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.
  41. Neuburger, S., Jansen, P., Heil, M., & Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2011). Gender differences in pre-adolescents' mental-rotation performance: Do they depend on grade and stimulus type? Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1238-1242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.017
  42. Ofte, S. H., & Hugdahl, K. (2002). Rightleft discrimination in male and female, young and old subjects. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(1), 82-92. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.1.82.966
  43. Olson, D. R., & Bialystok, E. (1983). Spatial cognition : The structure and development of mental representations of spatial relations. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  44. Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., & Riviere, J. (2014). Self-locomotion and spatial language and spatial cognition: Insights from typical and atypical development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 521. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00521
  45. Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(70)90108-3
  46. Perham, F. (1978). An investigation into the effect of instruction on the acquisition of transformation geometry concepts in first grade children and subsequent transfer to general spatial ability. In R. Lesh & D. Mierkiewicz (Eds.), Concerning the development of spatial and geometric concepts (pp. 229-241). Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.
  47. Piaget, J. (1956). Judgement and reasoning in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  48. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
  49. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1971). Mental imagery in the child : A study of the development of imaginal representation. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  50. Rosser, R. A., Ensing, S. S., Glider, P. J., & Lane, S. (1984). An information-processing analysis of children’s accuracy in predicting the appearance of rotated stimuli. Child Development, 55(6), 2204-2211. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129792
  51. Sholl, M. J., & Egeth, H. E. (1981). Right-left confusion in the adult: A verbal labeling effect. Memory & Cognition, 9(4), 339-350. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03197558
  52. Snyder, T. J. (1991). Self-rated right-left confusability and objectively measured right-left discrimination. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(2), 219-230. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649109540489
  53. Song, Y. S. (2002). A comparative study on the space concept of young children: Centering around Montessori program and general program (Unpublished master's thesis). Sungshin Women's University, Seoul, Korea.
  54. Swanson, R., & Benton, A. L. (1955). Some aspects of the genetic development of right-left discrimination. Child Development, 26(2), 123-133. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126294
  55. Vasilyeva, M., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Development of spatial cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 349-362. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1171
  56. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250-270. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250
  57. Wiedenbauer, G., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Manual training of mental rotation in children. Learning and Instruction, 18(1), 30-41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.009
  58. Worthington, R. K., Typpo, M., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. (1980). Spatial concept learning in preschool children: Motoric experiences and verbal repetition as adjuncts to passive listening. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50(1), 183-186. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1980.50.1.183
  59. Yoon, K. S. (1985). A study on the development of orientation concepts in the young children (Unpublished master's thesis). Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea.
  60. Yun, K. H. (1991). A study on the development of the child's (five to eleven year-old) conception of space (Unpublished master's thesis). Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea.