DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Asymmetric effect of aging on cognitive control processes: An ERP study

인지적 통제 과정에 미치는 노화의 비대칭적 영향: ERP 연구

  • Jin, Youngsun (Department of Psychology, Kyunpook National University) ;
  • Kim, Hyunok (Department of Psychology, Kyunpook National University)
  • Received : 2017.11.23
  • Accepted : 2017.12.05
  • Published : 2017.12.30

Abstract

Recently, studies on cognitive control revealed that the optimal level of control is determined on the basis of reward and cost. The value of reward can be subjective and therefore, the optimal control strength can vary accordingly. The inconsistent effect of aging on cognitive control can be the result of flexible adjustment of control signal strength made by the older subjects. In other words, the elderly people maintains the ability to set the optimal level of control, which is known as the function of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. On the other hand, the age-related decline in cognitive control is obvious in rule maintenance and inhibition, which has to do with the function of lateral prefrontal cortex. In this study, we had young and old adults perform go-no go task and compared the behavioral and neural results for different reward conditions. Both age groups showed the best performance and the largest ERN amplitude when the reward was most appealing to them. And there was no age effect in ERN amplitude even though older adults' d' and accuracy was inferior to younger participants. These findings suggest that the effect aging on different cognitive control processes can be asymmetric.

최근 인지적 통제에 대한 연구들은 최적의 통제 수준이 보상과 비용에 의해 결정된다는 것을 밝혀냈다. 보상이 가지는 가치는 주관적이며 이에 따라 최적의 신호 강도도 달라진다. 우리는 인지적 통제에 대한 노화의 영향에 대한 연구 결과들이 비일관적인 이유가 노인이 탄력적으로 통제 신호를 조절한 결과일 수 있다고 보았다. 다시 말해, 노인은 최적의 통제 수준을 결정하는 능력을 잘 유지하고 있을 수 있는데, 이는 전대상회의 역할로 알려져 있다. 반면 인지적 통제에서 노화에 따른 기능의 감퇴는 주로 규칙 표상의 유지와 억제에서 나타나는데, 이는 주로 외측전전두피질이 담당하고 있다. 본 연구는 청년과 노인에게 보상의 유형을 조절한 Go-Nogo 과제를 수행하게 하여 각 보상 조건에서 행동결과와 ERN(Error-Related Negativity) 크기를 비교하였다. 두 집단 모두는 각 연령에게 가장 중요하다고 여겨지는 조건에서 가장 뛰어난 행동 수행과 가장 큰 ERN 크기를 나타내었다. 또한 노인은 d'과 정확률에서는 연령차를 나타내었으나 ERN의 크기에서는 연령차를 나타내지 않았다. 이와 같은 결과는 인지적 통제의 각 과정에 대한 노화의 영향이 상이할 수 있을 가능성을 보여준다.

Keywords

References

  1. 김현옥, 진영선 (2016). The effect of reward type on cognitive control in young and old adults. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 29(4), 37-59.
  2. 진영선, 김현옥 (2016). 노화와 정서조절: 보상평가를 통한 인지적 통제의 탄력적 개입. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 29(3), 135-163.
  3. Ashley, V., Honzel, N., Larsen, J., Justus, T., & Swick, D. (2013). Attentional bias for trauma-related words: exaggerated emotional Stroop effect in Afghanistan and Iraq war veterans with PTSD. Bio Med Central Psychiatry, 13(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-1
  4. Barch, D., Braver, T., Racine, C., & Satpute, A. (2001). Cognitive control deficits in healthy aging:Neuroimaging investigations. Neuroimage, 13(6), S1025-S1025. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(01)92360-2
  5. Belanger, S., Belleville, S., & Gauthier, S. (2010). Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging: effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 581-590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.021
  6. Boehler, C. N., Schevernels, H., Hopf, J. M., Stoppel, C. M., & Krebs, R. M. (2014). Reward prospect rapidly speeds up response inhibition via reactive control. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 593-609.
  7. Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402(6758), 179-181. https://doi.org/10.1038/46035
  8. Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex:an update. Trends Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539-546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003
  9. Braver, T. S., & Barch, D. A. (2002). A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 809-817. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00067-2
  10. Braver, T. S., & West, R. (2008). Working memory, executive control, and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 311-372). New York, NY:Psychology Press.
  11. Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215-222. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2
  12. Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 331-338. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331
  13. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165-181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165
  14. Czernochowski, D. (2014). Conflict Monitoring Across the Life Span How to Tell Right From Wrong and Act Accordingly. Journal of Psychophysiology, 28(3), 124-135. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000120
  15. Dixon, M. L., & Christoff, K. (2012). The Decision to Engage Cognitive Control Is Driven by Expected Reward-Value: Neural and Behavioral Evidence. PLoS One, 7(12), e51637 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051637
  16. Dreher, J.C., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Kohn, P., Berman, K.F. (2008). Age-related changes in midbrain dopaminergic regulation of the human reward system. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105(39), 15106-15111 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0802127105
  17. Eppinger, B., Kray, J., Mock, B., & Mecklinger, A. (2008). Better or worse than expected? Aging, learning, and the ERN. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 521-539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.001
  18. Ford, J. H., & Kensinger, E. A. (2014). The relation between structural and functional connectivity depends on age and on task goals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 307
  19. Gehring, W. J., Himle, J., & Nisenson, L. G. (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Science, 11(1), 1-6. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00206
  20. Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. H. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93(1), 149-157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.007
  21. Grundler, T. O., Cavanagh, J. F., Figueroa, C. M., Frank, M. J., & Allen, J. J. (2009). Task-related dissociation in ERN amplitude as a function of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Neuropsychologia, 47(8-9), 1978-1987. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.010
  22. Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Brickman, A. M., Cheng, J. C., & Alexopoulos, G. S. (2009). Aging of cerebral white matter: a review of MRI findings. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(2), 109-117. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2087
  23. Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Gur, R. C., Perkins, A. C., Schroeder, L., Turner, T., Turetsky, B. I., Gur, R. E. (2003). Age-related differences in brain activation during emotional face processing. Neurobiology of Aging, 24(2), 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00099-4
  24. Hajcak, G. (2012). What We've Learned From Mistakes: Insights From Error-Related Brain Activity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 101-106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412436809
  25. Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(2), 87-96. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1323
  26. Hertel, P. T., & Gerstle, M. (2003). Depressive deficits in forgetting. Psychological Science, 14(6), 573-578. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1467.x
  27. Hess, T. M. (2014). Selective engagement of cognitive resources: motivational influences on older adults’ cognitive functioning. Psychological Science, 9(4), 388-407.
  28. Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(19), 8871-8876. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1002007107
  29. Jimura, K., Myerson, J., Hilgard, J., Keighley, J., Braver, T. S., & Green, L. (2011). Domain independence and stability in young and older adults' discounting of delayed rewards. Behavioral Processes, 87(3), 253-259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.04.006
  30. Kalanthroff, E., Henik, A., Derakshan, N., & Usher, M. (2015). Anxiety, Emotional Distraction, and Attentional Control in the Stroop Task. Emotion, 16(3), 293-300.
  31. Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 139(4), 665-682. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020198
  32. Krebs, R. M., Boehler, C. N., Appelbaum, L. G., & Woldorff, M. G. (2013). Reward associations reduce behavioral interference by changing the temporal dynamics of conflict processing. PLoS One, 8(1), e53894. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053894
  33. Lucci, G., Berchicci, M., Spinelli, D., Taddei, F., & Di Russo, F. (2013). The effects of aging on conflict detection. PLoS One, 8(2), e56566. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056566
  34. MacDonald, A. W., 3rd, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835-1838. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
  35. Mansouri, F. A., Tanaka, K., & Buckley, M. J. (2009). Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 141-152.
  36. Mathalon, D. H., Bennett, A., Askari, N., Gray, E. M., Rosenbloom, M. J., & Ford, J. M. (2003). Response-monitoring dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer's disease: an event-related potential study. Neurobiology of Aging, 24(5), 675-685. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00154-9
  37. Mathews, C. A., Perez, V. B., Delucchi, K. L., & Mathalon, D. H. (2012). Error-related negativity in individuals with obsessive-compulsive symptoms: toward an understanding of hoarding behaviors. Biological Psychology, 89(2), 487-494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.018
  38. Metzler-Baddeley, C., Jones, D. K., Steventon, J., Westacott, L., Aggleton, J. P., & O'Sullivan, M. J. (2012). Cingulum Microstructure Predicts Cognitive Control in Older Age and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(49), 17612-17619. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3299-12.2012
  39. Milham, M. P., Erickson, K. I., Banich, M. T., Kramer, A. F., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., & Cohen, N. J. (2002). Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 277-296. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1501
  40. Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 542-553 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542
  41. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
  42. Ochsner, K. N., Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion, Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(5), 242-249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010
  43. Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3419-3432. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00011
  44. Pailing, P. E., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2004). The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure:motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 84-95. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00124
  45. Pardo, J. V., Lee, J. T., Sheikh, S. A., Surerus-Johnson, C., Shah, H., Munch, K. R., Dysken, M. W. (2007). Where the brain grows old: decline in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal function with normal aging. Neuroimage, 35(3), 1231-1237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.044
  46. Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cereb Cortex, 18(5), 1010-1028. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm135
  47. Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (1998). Aging, intelligence, and anatomical segregation in the frontal lobes. Learning and Individual Differences, 10(3), 217-243. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80131-9
  48. Pietschmann, M., Endrass, T., Czerwon, B., & Kathmann, N. (2011). Aging, probabilistic learning and performance monitoring. Biological Psychology, 86(1), 74-82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.10.009
  49. Pontifex, M., Scudder, M., Brown, M., O'Leary, K., Wu, C., Themanson, J., et al. (2010). On the number of trials necessary for stabilization of error related brain activity across the life span, Psycholophysiology, 47(4), 767-773.
  50. Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik and Salthouse T. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Aging and Cognition (2nd edition, pp. 1-90). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  51. Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1676-1689. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi044
  52. Riesel, A., Weinberg, A., Endrass, T., Meyer, A., & Hajcak, G. (2013). The ERN is the ERN is the ERN? Convergent validity of error-related brain activity across different tasks. Biological Psychology, 93(3), 377-385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.007
  53. Salthouse, T. A. (2012). Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 201-226. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328
  54. Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Robertson, E. R., Mikels, J. A., Carstensen, L. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Selective attention to emotion in the aging brain. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), 519-529. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016952
  55. Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The Integration of Negative Affect, Pain, and Cognitive Control in the Cingulate Cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(3), 154-167. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2994
  56. Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217-240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
  57. Schott, B. H., Niehaus, L., Wittmann, B. C., et al. (2007). Ageing and early-stage Parkinson’s disease affect separable neural mechanisms of mesolimbic reward processing. Brain, 130(9), 2412-2424. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm147
  58. Stahl, J., Acharki, M., Kresimon, M., Voller, F., & Gibbons, H. (2015). Perfect error processing:Perfectionism-related variations in action monitoring and error processing mechanisms. Internationl Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(2), 153-162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.06.002
  59. Tisserand, D. J., Pruessner, J. C., Arigita, E. J. S., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Evans, A. C., Jolles, J., & Uylings, H. B. M. (2002). Regional frontal cortical volumes decrease differentially in aging: An MRI study to compare volumetric approaches and voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage, 17(2), 657-669. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1173
  60. Troyer, A. K., Leach, L., & Strauss, E. (2006). Aging and response inhibition: Normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(1), 20-35. https://doi.org/10.1080/138255890968187
  61. Vallesi, A. (2011). Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: A go/nogo event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 487, 313-317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.046
  62. Vanderhasselt, M. A., Baeken, C., Van Schuerbeek, P., Luypaert, R., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Inter-individual differences in the habitual use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are associated with variations in prefrontal cognitive control for emotional information: an event related fMRI study. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 433-439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.005
  63. Verhaeghen, P., & Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 849-857. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00071-4
  64. Weinberg, A., Olver, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2010), Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 85, 472-480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.011
  65. Weinberg, A., Riesel, A., & Hajcak, G. (2012), Integrating multiple perspectives on error-related brain activity: The ERN as a neural indicator of trait defensive reacrivity. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 84-100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9269-y
  66. West, R., & Alain, C. (2000). Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 179-189. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720179
  67. West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272-292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272
  68. Westbrook, A., Kester, D., & Braver, T. S. (2013). What Is the Subjective Cost of Cognitive Effort? Load, Trait, and Aging Effects Revealed by Economic Preference. PLoS One, 8(7), e68210. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068210
  69. Westbrook, A., & Braver, S. (2013). The economics of cognitive effort. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 704-726. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13001179