DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

The Actions Used by Children's and Their Underlying Theories whilst Engaged in Balance Tasks

무게중심 찾기 과제 수행과정에서 나타난 아동의 행위와 이론

  • Lee, Kwi Ok (Department of Children development and Education studies, Dongduk Women's University) ;
  • Woo, Nam Hee (Department of Children development and Education studies, Dongduk Women's University)
  • 이귀옥 (동덕여자대학교 아동학과) ;
  • 우남희 (동덕여자대학교 아동학과)
  • Received : 2012.08.31
  • Accepted : 2012.12.08
  • Published : 2012.12.31

Abstract

This study attempted to analyze in detail the actions used by children and to uncover the theories used by those children whilst engaged in solving balance tasks. Sixty children, aged between 3 to 6 from "H" child care center located in Seoul were selected as the subjects. The children were asked to balance 8 different blocks by putting them on a bar one by one. Two of the 8 blocks were balanced by the center of the length of the block, two were unbalanced by the center of the length because another block is glued on the side of the bottom block, three blocks were unbalanced due to the insertion of a piece of metal in the side of the blocks, and one was completely unbalanced because it consisted of three layers of blocks glued obliquely. Fifteen actions undertaken by the children in solving the tasks were analyzed and divided into 6 categories : place, turning, push, press, support, and others. Children used three theories which were 'no theories', 'length centered theory' and 'considered both length and weight theory' whilst engaged in balance tasks.

Keywords

References

  1. Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press.
  2. Bradmetz, J., & Amiotte-Suchet, H. (2001). The acquisition of a theory of action and the dissociation between action intention and outcome intention in children aged 2-6 years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(1), 37-49. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250042000041
  3. Dominowski, R. L. & Bource, Jr., (1994). History of research on thinking and problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg (Eds). Thinking and problem solving, pp. 1-35. San Diego : Academic Press.
  4. Ferretii, R. P., & Butterfield, E. C. (1985). The classification of children's knowledge : Development on the balance-scale and inclinedplane task. Journal of experimental child psychology. 39, 131-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90033-5
  5. Fosnot, C. T., Forman, G. E., Edwards, C. P., & Goldhaber, J. (1988). The development of an understanding of balance and the effect of training via stop-action video. Journal of applied developmental psychology, 9(1), 1-26.
  6. Gelman, R. (1978). Cognitive development. Annual Review of Psychology, 29(1), 297-332. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.001501
  7. Ginsberg, H. P., & Opper, S. (1988). Piaget's theory of intellectual development. 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs, N. J. : Prentice-Hall.
  8. Gopnik, A. (1988). Conceptual and sematic development as theory change : the case of object permanence. Mind & Language, 3(3), 197-216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1988.tb00143.x
  9. Gopnik, A., & Melzoff, A. N.(1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press.
  10. Gopnik, A., Melzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2001). The Scientist in the crib : What early learning tells us about the mind. New York : Perennial.
  11. Jansen, B. R. J., & Maas, H. L. J. (2002). The development of children's rule use on the balance scale task. Journal of experimental child psychology, 81(4), 383-416. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2664
  12. Kamii, C., & Devries, R. (1978). Physical konwldege in preschool education : implications of Piaget's theory. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood cliffs, New Jersey.
  13. Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1974). If you want to get ahead, get a theory. Cognition, 3(3), 195-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(74)90008-0
  14. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). Getting developmental differences of studying child development? Cognition, 10(1), 151-158. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(81)90039-1
  15. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1984). Children's problem solving. In Lamb, M. E., Brown, Ann L., & Rogoff, B. (Eds). Advances in developmental psychology, 3(1), pp. 39-90. London : Lawence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
  16. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1988). The child is theoretician, not an inductivist. Mind and Language, 3(3), 183-195. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1988.tb00142.x
  17. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity : A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press.
  18. Kim, Y., Kim, H., & Kim, M. (2010). Design requirements of mediating device for total physical response- A protocol analysis of preschool children's behavioral patterns. Korean Journal of the science of Emotion & sensibility, 13(1), 103-110.
  19. Kitcher, P. (1988). The child as parent of the scientist. Mind and language, 3(3), 217-228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1988.tb00144.x
  20. Messer, D., Norgate, S., Joiner, R., & Littleton, K. (1996). Development without learning? Educational Psychology, 16(1), 5-19. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341960160101
  21. Metz, K. E. (1993). Preschooler's developing knowledge of the pan balance : From new presentation to transformed problem solving. Cognition and Instruction, 11(1), 31-93. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1101_2
  22. Murphy, G. L., & Medin, Douglas, L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological review, 92(3), 289-316. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.3.289
  23. Piaget, J. (1976). The grasp of consciousnes : Action and concept in the young child, trans. S. Wedgwood. Cambridge, Harvard University Press.
  24. Piaget, J. (1978). Success and understand, trans. A. J. Pomerans. Cambridge, Mass : Harvard University Press.
  25. Pine, K. J., & Messer, D J. (1999). What children do and what children know : Looking beyond success using Karmiloff-Smith's RR framework. New Ideas in Psychology, 17(1), 17-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0732-118X(98)00014-2
  26. Pine, K. J., & Messer, D J. (1998). Group collaboration effects and the explicitness of children's knowledge. Cognitive Development, 13(1), 109-126. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90023-5
  27. Pine , K. J., & Messer, D J. (2000). The effect of explaining another's actions on children's implicit theories of balance. Cognition and Instruction, 18(1), 35-51. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532690XCI1801_02
  28. Pine, K. J., Messer, D. J., & Godfrey, K. (1999). The teachability of children with naive theories : An exploration of the effects of two teaching methods. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(2), 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709999157671
  29. Pine, K. J., Messer, D. J., & John, K. St. (2002). Children's learning from contrast modeling. Cognitive Development, 17, 1249-1263. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00114-4
  30. Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(2), 1-84. https://doi.org/10.2307/1165995
  31. Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2002). Development of rules and strategies : Balancing the old and the new. Journal of Experimental child Psychology, 81, 446-457. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2666
  32. Turner, G. F. W., & Thomas, H. (2002). Briding the gap between theory and model : A reflection on the balance scale task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 466-481. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2668
  33. Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press.
  34. Zelazo, P. D., & Muller, U. (2002). The balance beam in the balance : Reflections on rules, relational complexity, and developmental processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 458-465. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2667