Understanding of mind and social skills in adolescents

청소년의 마음이해 능력과 사회적 능력

  • Hyeon Ok Choi (Developmental of Psychology, Chungbuk National University) ;
  • Hei Rhee Ghim (Developmental of Psychology, Chungbuk National University)
  • Received : 2008.01.16
  • Published : 2008.02.28

Abstract

The present study was to investigate the developmental change of adolescent's theory of mind. In addition, the relations between theory of mind and the social competence were investigated. Sixty students in each the 6th and 8th grade group were participated in this study. Adolescents' understanding of mind were measured by four kinds of mindreading tasks; the second-order false belief tasks, understanding the ambiguous social behaviors tasks, understading the hidden meaning of a figurative statement tasks, and the "faux pas" tasks. Social skills were measured by social skill scales. Eighth graders performed better than the 6th graders on the understanding the ambiguous social behaviors tasks and faux pas tasks but not on the second-order false belief tasks and understanding the hidden meaning of figurative statement tasks. The results suggests that the mindreading ability continues to develop during the adolescence, especially the ability to interpret others' ambiguous social behaviors by reading their mental states and the ability to understand that a person says a faux pas it is due to a mistaken belief. In addition, the understanding the ambiguous social behaviors tasks was found to be the best task to predict social behaviors. The results suggests that the mindreading ability would be a significant explanatory factor on social competence.

본 연구는 청소년기에도 마음이해 능력이 계속 발달하는지, 또 마음이해 능력이 사회적 능력과 관련되는지 알아보기 위해 수행되었다. 초등학교 6학년 60명과 중학교 2학년 60명이 연구에 참여하였다. 마음이해 능력은 2차 순위 틀린 믿음 과제, 애매한 행동 이해하기 과제, 숨은 말뜻 이해하기 과제, 그리고 헛디딤 말 이해하기 과제로 측정하였다. 사회적 능력은 마음이해 관련 사회행동과 마음이해와 무관할 수 있는 관습적 사회행동을 묻는 질문지로 측정하였다. 2차 순위 틀린 믿음과제와 숨은 말뜻 이해하기 과제에서는 두 집단 간의 수행차가 나타나지 않았으나 애매한 행동 이해하기 과제와 헛디딤 말 이해하기 과제에서는 중학생의 수행이 더 높았다. 이는 마음읽기 능력이 청소년기에도 계속 발달하는데, 특히 애매한 행동의 의미를 이해하는 능력과 말하는 이가 상대방에 대한 특정한 사실을 알지 못해 실수로 하는 헛디딤 말이 상대방의 기분을 상하게 한다는 사실을 이해하는 능력이 6학년 이후에도 발달함을 보여준다. 또, 여러 마음이해 과제 중에서, 애매한 행동 이해하기가 사회적 행동을 가장 잘 예측하는 것으로 나타났으며, 마음이해 과제들의 사회적 행동 설명력 또한 크지는 않았지만 유의미한 것으로 나타났다.

Keywords

Acknowledgement

이 논문(도서, 작품)은 2006학년도 충북대학교 학술연구지원사업의 연구비지원에 의하여 연구되었음.

References

  1. 김혜리, 구재선, 김경미, 양혜영, 정명숙, 최현옥, 이수미 (2007). 학령기와 그 이후의 마음이해 발달: 애매한 상황에서의 마음읽기 능력. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 20(1), 19-47.
  2. 김혜리, 이숙희 (2005). 인기있는 아동은 마음읽기를 잘하나. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 18(1), 1-18.
  3. 박영신, 김의철 (2004). 청소년의 인간관계와 일탈행동: 부모자녀관계, 친구관계, 교사 학생관계를 중심으로. 한국심리학회지: 사회문제, 10, 87-115
  4. 한종철, 김인경 (2000). 또래따돌림과 심리사회적 부적응. 한국심리학회지: 사회문제, 6(2), 103-114.
  5. Astington, J. W. (1991). Intention in the child's theory of mind. In D. Frye and C. Moore (Eds), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum
  6. Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1995). Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 151-165.
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., O'Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K., (1999). Recognition of Faux Pas by Normally Developing Children and Children with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental disorders, 29(5), 407-418
  9. Capage, L., & Watson, A. C. (2001). Individual differences in theory of mind. aggressive behavior, and social skills in young children. Early Education & Development, 12(4), 613-628
  10. Carpendale, J. I., Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the Distinction between False Belief Understanding and Subscribing to an interpretive Theory of Mind. Child development, 67(4), 1686-1706
  11. Cuming, S., & Repacholi, B. (1999). Is there a link between children's peer relationships and their theory of mind? Poster presented at the 11th Australian Human Development Association Conference, Sydney, Australia.
  12. Dunn, J. (1995). Children as psychologists: The later correlates of individual differences in understanding emotions and other minds. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 187-201
  13. Happe, F. (1994). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of Relevance Theory. Cognition, 48, 101-119.
  14. Hogrefe, G. J., Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance and false belief: A developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states. Child Development, 57, 567-582
  15. Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: on the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 167-185.
  16. Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-old's difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125-129
  17. Perner, J., & Wimmer, H., (1985). Attribution of Second-Order Beliefs by 5- to 10-year-Old Children. Journal of Experimental Children Psychology, 39, 437-471
  18. Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 545-564.
  19. Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A., & Capage, L. (1999). Social interacion skills and theory of mind in young children. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 386-391
  20. Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children's reasoning about beliefs. Cognition, 30, 239-277.
  21. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.
  22. Yuill, N., & Pearson, A. (1998). The development of bases for trait attribution: Children's understanding of traits as casual mechanism based on desire. Developmental Psychology, 34, 574-586.